III.
The Annual Sexual Rhythm--In Animals--In Man--Tendency
of the Sexual Impulse to become Heightened in Spring
and Autumn--The Prevalence of Seasonal Erotic Festivals--The
Feast of Fools--The Easter and Midsummer Bonfires--The
Seasonal Variations in Birthrate--The Causes of those
Variations--The Typical Conception-rate Curve for Europe--The
Seasonal Periodicity of Seminal Emissions During Sleep--Original
Observations--Spring and Autumn the Chief Periods of
Involuntary Sexual Excitement--The Seasonal Periodicity
of Rapes--Of Outbreaks among Prisoners--The Seasonal
Curves of Insanity and Suicide--The Growth of Children
According to Season--The Annual Curve of Bread-consumption
in Prisons--Seasonal Periodicity of Scarlet Fever--The
Underlying Causes of these Seasonal Phenomena.
That there are annual seasonal changes in the human
organism, especially connected with the sexual function,
is a statement that has been made by physiologists and
others from time to time, and the statement has even
reached the poets, who have frequently declared that
spring is the season of love.
Thus, sixty years ago, Laycock, an acute pioneer in
the investigation of the working of the human organism,
brought together (in a chapter on "The Periodic
Movements in the Reproductive Organs of Woman,"
in his _Nervous Diseases of Women_, 1840, pp. 61-70)
much interesting evidence to show that the system undergoes
changes about the vernal and autumnal equinoxes, and
that these changes are largely sexual.
Edward Smith, also a notable pioneer in this field of
human periodicity, and, indeed, the first to make definite
observations on a number of points bearing on it, sums
up, in his remarkable book, _Health and Disease as Influenced
by Daily, Seasonal, and Other Cyclical Changes in the
Human System_ (1861), to the effect that season is a
more powerful influence on the system than temperature
or atmospheric pressure; "in the early and middle
parts of spring every function of the body is in its
highest degree of efficiency," while autumn is
"essentially a period of change from the minimum
toward the maximum of vital conditions." He found
that in April and May most carbonic acid is evolved,
there being then a progressive diminution to September,
and then a progressive increase; the respiratory rate
also fell from a maximum in April to a minimum maintained
at exactly the same level throughout August, September,
October, and November; spring was found to be the season
of maximum, autumn of minimum, muscular power; sensibility
to tactile and temperature impressions was also greater
in spring.
Kulischer, studying the sexual customs of various human
races, concluded that in primitive times, only at two
special seasons--at spring and in harvest-time--did
pairing take place; and that, when pairing ceased to
be strictly confined to these periods, its symbolical
representation was still so confined, even among the
civilized nations of Europe. He further argued that
the physiological impulse was only felt at these periods.
(Kulischer, "Die geschlechtliche Zuchtwahl bei
den Menschen in der Urzeit," _Zeitschrift fuer
Ethnologie_, 1876, pp. 152 and 157.) Cohnstein ("Ueber
Praedilectionszeiten bei Schwangerschaft," _Archiv
fuer Gynaekologie_, 1879) also suggested that women
sometimes only conceive at certain periods of the year.
Wiltshire, who made various interesting observations
regarding the physiology of menstruation, wrote: "Many
years ago, I concluded that every women had a law peculiar
to herself, which governed the times of her bringing
forth (and conceiving); that she was more prone to bring
forth at certain epochs than at others; and subsequent
researches have established the accuracy of the forecast."
He further stated his belief in a "primordial seasonal
aptitude for procreation, the impress of which still
remains, and, to some extent, governs the breeding-times
of humanity." (A. Wiltshire, "Lectures on
the Comparative Physiology of Menstruation," _British
Medical Journal_, March, 1883, pp. 502, etc.)
Westermarck, in a chapter of his _History of Human Marriage_,
dealing with the question of "A Human Pairing Season
in Primitive Times," brings forward evidence showing
that spring, or, rather, early summer, is the time for
increase of the sexual instinct, and argues that this
is a survival of an ancient pairing season; spring,
he points out, is a season of want, rather than abundance,
for a frugivorous species, but when men took to herbs,
roots, and animal food, spring became a time of abundance,
and suitable for the birth of children. He thus considers
that in man, as in lower animals, the times of conception
are governed by the times most suitable for birth.
Rosenstadt, as we shall see later, also believes that
men to-day have inherited a physiological custom of
procreating at a certain epoch, and he thus accounts
for the seasonal changes in the birthrate.
Heape, who also believes that "at one period of
its existence the human species had a special breeding
season," follows Wiltshire in suggesting that "there
is some reason to believe that the human female is not
always in a condition to breed." (W. Heape, "Menstruation
and Ovulation of _Macacus rhesus_," _Philosophical
Transactions_, 1897; id. "The Sexual Season of
Mammals," _Quarterly Journal Microscopical Science_,
1900.)
Except, however, in one important respect, with which
we shall presently have to deal, few attempts have been
made to demonstrate any annual organic sexual rhythm.
The supposition of such annual cycle is usually little
more than a deduction from the existence of the well-marked
seasonal sexual rhythm in animals. Most of the higher
animals breed only once or twice a year, and at such
a period that the young are born when food is most plentiful.
At other periods the female is incapable of breeding,
and without sexual desires, while the male is either
in the same condition or in a condition of latent sexuality.
Under the influence of domestication, animals tend to
lose the strict periodicity of the wild condition, and
become apt for breeding at more frequent intervals.
Thus among dogs in the wild state the bitch only experiences
heat once a year, in the spring. Among domesticated
dogs, there is not only the spring period of heat, early
in the year, but also an autumn period, about six months
later; the primitive period, however, remains the most
important one, and the best litters of pups are said
to be produced in the spring. The mare is in season
in spring and summer; sheep take the ram in autumn.[128]
Many of the menstruating monkeys also, whether or not
sexual desire is present throughout the year, only conceive
in spring and in autumn. Almost any time of the year
may be an animal's pairing season, this season being
apparently in part determined by the economic conditions
which will prevail at birth. While it is essential that
animals should be born during the season of greatest
abundance, it is equally essential that pairing, which
involves great expenditure of energy, should also take
place at a season of maximum physical vigor.
As an example of the sexual history of an animal through
the year, I may quote the following description, by
Dr. A.W. Johnstone, of the habits of the American deer:
"Our common American deer, in winter-time, is half-starved
for lack of vegetation in the woods; the low temperature,
snow, and ice, make his conditions of life harder for
lack of the proper amount of food, whereby he becomes
an easier prey to carnivorous animals. He has difficulty
even in preserving life. In spring he sheds his winter
coat, and is provided with a suit of lighter hair, and
while this is going on the male grows antlers for defence.
The female about this time is far along in pregnancy,
and when the antlers are fully grown she drops the fawn.
When the fawns are dropped vegetation is plentiful and
lactation sets in. During this time the male is kept
fully employed in getting food and guarding his more
or less helpless family. As the season advances the
vegetation increases and the fawn begins to eat grass.
When the summer heat commences the little streams begin
to dry up, and the animal once more has difficulty in
supporting life because of the enervating heat, the
effect of drought on the vegetation, and the distance
which has to be traveled to get water; therefore, fully
ten months in each year the deer has all he can do to
live without extra exertion incident to rutting. Soon
after the autumn rains commence vegetation becomes more
luxurious, the antlers of the male and new suits of
hair for both are fully grown, heat of the summer is
gone, food and drink are plentiful everywhere, the fawns
are weaned, and both sexes are in the very finest condition.
Then, and then only, in the whole year, comes the rut,
which, to them as to most other animals, means an unwonted
amount of physical exercise besides the everyday runs
for life from their natural enemies, and an unusual
amount of energy is used up. If a doe dislikes the attention
of a special buck, miles of racing result. If jealous
males meet, furious battles take place. The strain on
both sexes could not possibly be endured at any other
season of the year. With approach of cold weather, climatic
deprivations and winter dangers commence and rut closes.
In all wild animals, rut occurs only when the climatic
and other conditions favor the highest physical development.
This law holds good in all wild birds, for it is then
only that they can stand the strain incident to love-making.
The common American crow is a very good study. In the
winter he travels around the ricefields of the South,
leading a tramp's existence in a country foreign to
him, and to which he goes only to escape the rigors
of the northern climate. For several weeks in the spring
he goes about the fields, gathering up the worms and
grubs. After his long flight from the South he experiences
several weeks of an almost ideal existence, his food
is plentiful, he becomes strong and hearty, and then
he turns to thoughts of love. In the pairing season
he does more work than at any other time in the year:
fantastic dances, racing and chasing after the females,
and savage fights with rivals. He endures more than
would be possible in his ordinary physical state. Then
come the care of the young and the long flights for
water and food during the drought of the summer. After
the molt, autumn finds him once more in flock, and with
the first frosts he is off again to the South. In the
wild state, rut is the capstone of perfect physical
condition." (A.W. Johnstone, "The Relation
of Menstruation to the other Reproductive Functions,"
_American Journal of Obstetrics_, vol. xxxii, 1895.)
Wiltshire ("Lectures on the Comparative Physiology
of Menstruation," _British Medical Journal_, March,
1888) and Westermarck (_History of Human Marriage_,
Chapter II) enumerate the pairing season of a number
of different animals.
With regard to the breeding seasons of monkeys, little
seems to be positively known. Heape made special inquiries
with reference to the two species whose sexual life
he investigated. He was informed that _Semnopithecus
entellus_ breeds twice a year, in April and in October.
He accepts Aitcheson's statement that the _Macacus rhesus_,
in Simla, copulates in October, and adds that in the
very different climate of the plains it appears to copulate
in May. He concludes that the breeding season varies
greatly in dependence on climate, but believes that
the breeding season is always preserved, and that it
affects the sexual aptitude of the male. He could not
make his monkeys copulate during February or March,
but is unable to say whether or not sexual intercourse
is generally admitted outside the breeding season. He
quotes the observation of Breschet that monkeys copulate
during pregnancy.
In primitive human races we very frequently trace precisely
the same influence of the seasonal impulse as may be
witnessed in the higher animals, although among human
races it does not always result that the children are
born at the time of the greatest plenty, and on account
of the development of human skill such a result is not
necessary. Thus Dr. Cook found among the Eskimo that
during the long winter nights the secretions are diminished,
muscular power is weak, and the passions are depressed.
Soon after the sun appears a kind of rut affects the
young population. They tremble with the intensity of
sexual passion, and for several weeks much of the time
is taken up with courtship and love. Hence, the majority
of the children are born nine months later, when the
four months of perpetual night are beginning. A marked
seasonal periodicity of this kind is not confined to
the Arctic regions. We may also find it in the tropics.
In Cambodia, Mondiere has found that twice a year, in
April and September, men seem to experience a "veritable
rut," and will sometimes even kill women who resist
them.[129]
These two periods, spring and autumn--the season for
greeting the appearance of life and the season for reveling
in its final fruition--seem to be everywhere throughout
the world the most usual seasons for erotic festivals.
In classical Greece and Rome, in India, among the Indians
of North and South America, spring is the most usual
season, while in Africa the yam harvest of autumn is
the season chiefly selected. There are, of course, numerous
exceptions to this rule, and it is common to find both
seasons observed. Taking, indeed, a broad view of festivals
throughout the world, we may say that there are four
seasons when they are held: the winter solstice, when
the days begin to lengthen and primitive man rejoices
in the lengthening and seeks to assist it;[130] the
vernal equinox, the period of germination and the return
of life; the summer solstice, when the sun reaches its
height; and autumn, the period of fruition, of thankfulness,
and of repose. But it is rarely that we find a people
seriously celebrating more than two of these festival
seasons.
In Australia, according to Mueller as quoted by Ploss
and Bartels, marriage and conception take place during
the warm season, when there is greatest abundance of
food, and to some extent is even confined to that period.
Oldfield and others state that the Australian erotic
festivals take place only in spring. Among some tribes,
Mueller adds, such as the Watschandis, conception is
inaugurated by a festival called _kaaro_, which takes
place in the warm season at the first new moon after
the yams are ripe. The leading feature of this festival
is a moonlight dance, representing the sexual act symbolically.
With their spears, regarded as the symbols of the male
organ, the men attack bushes, which represent the female
organs. They thus work themselves up to a state of extreme
sexual excitement.[131] Among the Papuans of New Guinea,
also, according to Miklucho-Macleay, conceptions chiefly
occur at the end of harvest, and Guise describes the
great annual festival of the year which takes place
at the time of the yam and banana harvest, when the
girls undergo a ceremony of initiation and marriages
are effected.[132] In Central Africa, says Sir H.H.
Johnston, in his _Central Africa_, sexual orgies are
seriously entered into at certain seasons of the year,
but he neglects to mention what these seasons are. The
people of New Britain, according to Weisser (as quoted
by Ploss and Bartels), carefully guard their young girls
from the young men. At certain times, however, a loud
trumpet is blown in the evening, and the girls are then
allowed to go away into the bush to mix freely with
the young men. In ancient Peru (according to an account
derived from a pastoral letter of Archbishop Villagomez
of Lima), in December, when the fruit of the _paltay_
is ripe, a festival was held, preceded by a five days'
fast. During the festival, which lasted six days and
six nights, men and women met together in a state of
complete nudity at a certain spot among the gardens,
and all raced toward a certain hill. Every man who caught
up with a woman in the race was bound at once to have
intercourse with her.
Very instructive, from our present point of view, is
the account given by Dalton, of the festivals of the
various Bengal races. Thus the Hos (a Kolarian tribe),
of Bengal, are a purely agricultural people, and the
chief festival of the year with them is the _magh parah_.
It is held in the month of January, "when the granaries
are full of grain, and the people, to use their own
expression, full of devilry." It is the festival
of the harvest-home, the termination of the year's toil,
and is always held at full moon. The festival is a _saturnalia_,
when all rules of duty and decorum are forgotten, and
the utmost liberty is allowed to women and girls, who
become like bacchantes. The people believe that at this
time both men and women become overcharged with vitality,
and that a safety valve is absolutely necessary. The
festival begins with a religious sacrifice made by the
village priest or elders, and with prayers for the departed
and for the vouchsafing of seasonable rain and good
crops. The religious ceremonies over, the people give
themselves up to feasting and to drinking the home-made
beer, the preparation of which from fermented rice is
one of a girl's chief accomplishments. "The Ho
population," wrote Dalton, "are at other seasons
quiet and reserved in manner, and in their demeanor
toward women gentle and decorous; even in their flirtations
they never transcend the bounds of decency. The girls,
though full of spirits and somewhat saucy, have innate
notions of propriety that make them modest in demeanor,
though devoid of all prudery, and of the obscene abuse,
so frequently heard from the lips of common women in
Bengal, they appear to have no knowledge. They are delicately
sensitive under harsh language of any kind, and never
use it to others; and since their adoption of clothing
they are careful to drape themselves decently, as well
as gracefully; but they throw all this aside during
the _magh_ feast. Their nature appears to undergo a
temporary change. Sons and daughters revile their parents
in gross language, and parents their children; men and
women become almost like animals in the indulgence of
their amorous propensities. They enact all that was
ever portrayed by prurient artists in a bacchanalian
festival or pandean orgy; and as the light of the sun
they adore, and the presence of numerous spectators,
seems to be no restraint on their indulgence, it cannot
be expected that chastity is preserved when the shades
of night fall on such a scene of licentiousness and
debauchery." While, however, thus representing
the festival as a mere debauch, Dalton adds that relationships
formed at this time generally end in marriage. There
is also a flower festival in April and May, of religious
nature, but the dances at this festival are quieter
in character.[133]
In Burmah the great festival of the year is the full
moon of October, following the Buddhist Lent season
(which is also the wet season), during which there is
no sexual intercourse. The other great festival is the
New Year in March.[134]
In classical times the great festivals were held at
the same time as in northern and modern Europe. The
_brumalia_ took place in midwinter, when the days were
shortest, and the _rosalia_, according to early custom
in May or June, and at a later time about Easter. After
the establishment of Christianity the Church made constant
efforts to suppress this latter festival, and it was
referred to by an eighth century council as "a
wicked and reprehensible holiday-making." These
festivals appear to be intimately associated with Dionysus
worship, and the flower-festival of Dionysus, as well
as the Roman Liberales in honor of Bacchus, was celebrated
in March with worship of Priapus. The festivals of the
Delian Apollo and of Artemis, both took place during
the first week in May and the Roman Bacchanales in October.[135]
The mediaeval Feast of Fools was to a large extent a
seasonal orgy licensed by the Church. It may be traced
directly back through the barbatories of the lower empire
to the Roman _saturnalia_, and at Sens, the ancient
ecclesiastical metropolis of France, it was held at
about the same time as the _saturnalia_, on the Feast
of the Circumcision, i.e., New Year's Day. It was not,
however, always held at this time; thus at Evreux it
took place on the 1st of May.[136]
The Easter bonfires of northern-central Europe, the
Midsummer (St. John's Eve) fires of southern-central
Europe, still bear witness to the ancient festivals.[137]
There is certainly a connection between these bonfires
and erotic festivals; it is noteworthy that they occur
chiefly at the period of spring and early summer, which,
on other grounds, is widely regarded as the time for
the increase of the sexual instinct, while the less
frequent period for the bonfires is that of the minor
sexual climax. Mannhardt was perhaps the first to show
how intimately these spring and early summer festivals--held
with bonfires and dances and the music of violin--have
been associated with love-making and the choice of a
mate.[138] In spring, the first Monday in Lent (Quadrigesima)
and Easter Eve were frequent days for such bonfires.
In May, among the Franks of the Main, the unmarried
women, naked and adorned with flowers, danced on the
Blocksberg before the men, as described by Herbels in
the tenth century.[139] In the central highlands of
Scotland the Beltane fires were kindled on the 1st of
May. Bonfires sometimes took place on Halloween (October
31st) and Christmas. But the great season all over Europe
for these bonfires, then often held with erotic ceremonial,
is the summer solstice, the 23d of June, the eve of
Midsummer, or St. John's Day.[140]
The Bohemians and other Slavonic races formerly had
meetings with sexual license. This was so up to the
beginning of the sixteenth century on the banks of rivers
near Novgorod. The meetings took place, as a rule, the
day before the Festival of John the Baptist, which,
in pagan times, was that of a divinity known by the
name of Jarilo (equivalent to Priapus). Half a century
later, a new ecclesiastical code sought to abolish every
vestige of the early festivals held on Christmas Day,
on the Day of the Baptism, of Our Lord, and on John
the Baptist's Day. A general feature of all these festivals
(says Kowalewsky) was the prevalence of the promiscuous
intercourse of the sexes. Among the Ehstonians, at the
end of the eighteenth century, thousands of persons
would gather around an old ruined church (in the Fellinschen)
on the Eve of St. John, light a bonfire, and throw sacrificial
gifts into it. Sterile women danced naked among the
ruins; much eating and drinking went on, while the young
men and maidens disappeared into the woods to do what
they would. Festivals of this character still take place
at the end of June in some districts. Young unmarried
couples jump barefoot over large fires, usually near
rivers or ponds. Licentiousness is rare.[141] But in
many parts of Russia the peasants still attach little
value to virginity, and even prefer women who have been
mothers. The population of the Grisons in the sixteenth
century held regular meetings not less licentious than
those of the Cossacks. These were abolished by law.
Kowalewsky regards all such customs as a survival of
early forms of promiscuity.[142]
Frazer (_Golden Bough_, 2d ed., 1900, vol. iii, pp.
236-350) fully describes and discusses the dances, bonfires
and festivals of spring and summer, of Halloween (October
31), and Christmas. He also explains the sexual character
of these festivals. "There are clear indications,"
he observes (p. 305), "that even human fecundity
is supposed to be promoted by the genial heat of the
fires. It is an Irish belief that a girl who jumps thrice
over the midsummer bonfire will soon marry and become
the mother of many children; and in various parts of
France they think that if a girl dances round nine fires
she will be sure to marry within a year. On the other
hand, in Lechrain, people say that if a young man and
woman, leaping over the midsummer fire together, escape
unsmirched, the young woman will not become a mother
within twelve months--the flames have not touched and
fertilized her. The rule observed in some parts of France
and Belgium, that the bonfires on the first Sunday in
Lent should be kindled by the person who was last married,
seems to belong to the same class of ideas, whether
it be that such a person is supposed to receive from,
or impart to, the fire a generative and fertilizing
influence. The common practice of lovers leaping over
the fires hand-in-hand may very well have originated
in a notion that thereby their marriage would be more
likely to be blessed with offspring. And the scenes
of profligacy which appear to have marked the midsummer
celebration among the Ehstonians, as they once marked
the celebration of May Day among ourselves, may have
sprung, not from the mere license of holiday-makers,
but from a crude notion that such orgies were justified,
if not required, by some mysterious bond which linked
the life of man, to the courses of the heavens at the
turning-point of the year."
As regards these primitive festivals, although the evidence
is scattered and sometimes obscure, certain main conclusions
clearly emerge. In early Europe there were, according
to Grimm, only two seasons, sometimes regarded as spring
and winter, sometimes as spring and autumn, and for
mythical purposes these seasons were alone available.[143]
The appearance of each of these two seasons was inaugurated
by festivals which were religious and often erotic in
character. The Slavonic year began in March, at which
time there was formerly, it is believed, a great festival,
not only in Slavonic but also in Teutonic countries.
In Northern Germany there were Easter bonfires always
associated with mountains or hills. The Celtic bonfires
were held at the beginning of May, while the Teutonic
May-day, or _Walpurgisnacht_, is a very ancient sacred
festival, associated with erotic ceremonial, and regarded
by Grimm as having a common origin with the Roman _floralia_
and the Greek _dionysia_. Thus, in Europe, Grimm concludes:
"there are four different ways of welcoming summer.
In Sweden and Gothland a battle of winter and summer,
a triumphal entry of the latter. In Schonen, Denmark,
Lower Saxony, and England, simply May-riding, or fetching
of the May-wagon. On the Rhine merely a battle of winter
and summer, without immersion, without the pomp of an
entry. In Franconia, Thuringia, Meissen, Silesia, and
Bohemia only the carrying out of wintry death; no battle,
no formal introduction of summer. Of these festivals
the first and second fall in May, the third and fourth
in March. In the first two, the whole population take
part with unabated enthusiasm; in the last two only
the lower poorer class.... Everything goes to prove
that the approach of summer was to our forefathers a
holy tide, welcomed by sacrifice, feast, and dance,
and largely governing and brightening the people's life."[144]
The early spring festival of March, the festival of
Ostara, the goddess of spring, has become identified
with the Christian festival of Resurrection (just as
the summer solstice festival has been placed beneath
the patronage of St. John the Baptist); but there has
been only an amalgamation of closely-allied rites, for
the Christian festival also may be traced back to a
similar origin. Among the early Arabians the great _ragab_
feast, identified by Ewald and Robertson Smith with
the Jewish _paschal_ feast, fell in the spring or early
summer, when the camels and other domestic animals brought
forth their young and the shepherds offered their sacrifices.[145]
Babylonia, the supreme early centre of religious and
cosmological culture, presents a more decisive example
of the sex festival. The festival of Tammuz is precisely
analogous to the European festival of St. John's Day.
Tammuz was the solar god of spring vegetation, and closely
associated with Ishtar, also an agricultural deity of
fertility. The Tammuz festival was, in the earliest
times, held toward the summer solstice, at the time
of the first wheat and barley harvest. In Babylonia,
as in primitive Europe, there were only two seasons;
the festival of Tammuz, coming at the end of winter
and the beginning of summer, was a fast followed by
a feast, a time of mourning for winter, of rejoicing
for summer. It is part of the primitive function of
sacred ritual to be symbolical of natural processes,
a mysterious representation of natural processes with
the object of bringing them about.[146] The Tammuz festival
was an appeal to the powers of Nature to exhibit their
generative functions; its erotic character is indicated
not only by the well-known fact that the priestesses
of Ishtar (the Kadishtu, or "holy ones") were
prostitutes, but by the statements in Babylonian legends
concerning the state of the earth during Ishtar's winter
absence, when the bull, the ass, and man ceased to reproduce.
It is evident that the return of spring, coincident
with the Tammuz festival, was regarded as the period
for the return of the reproductive instinct even in
man.[147] So that along this line also we are led back
to a great procreative festival.
Thus the great spring festivals were held between March
and June, frequently culminating in a great orgy on
Midsummer's Eve. The next great season of festivals
in Europe was in autumn. The beginning of August was
a great festival in Celtic lands, and the echoes of
it, Rhys remarks, have not yet died out in Wales.[148]
The beginning of November, both in Celtic and Teutonic
countries, was a period of bonfires.[149] In Germanic
countries especially there was a great festival at the
time. The Germanic year began at Martinmas (November
11th), and the great festival of the year was then held.
It is the oldest Germanic festival on record, and retained
its importance even in the Middle Ages. There was feasting
all night, and the cattle that were to be killed were
devoted to the gods; the goose was associated with this
festival.[150] These autumn festivals culminated in
the great festival of the winter solstice which we have
perpetuated in the celebrations of Christmas and New
Year. Thus, while the two great primitive culminating
festivals of spring and autumn correspond exactly (as
we shall see) with the seasons of maximum fecundation,
even in the Europe of to-day, the earlier spring (March)
and--though less closely--autumn (November) festivals
correspond with the periods of maximum spontaneous sexual
disturbance, as far as I have been able to obtain precise
evidence of such disturbance. That the maximum of physiological
sexual excitement should tend to appear earlier than
the maximum of fecundation is a result that might be
expected.
The considerations so far brought forward clearly indicate
that among primitive races there are frequently one
or two seasons in the year--especially spring and autumn--during
which sexual intercourse is chiefly or even exclusively
carried on, and they further indicate that these primitive
customs persist to some extent even in Europe to-day.
It would still remain, to determine whether any such
influence affects the whole mass of the civilized population
and determines the times at which intercourse, or fecundation,
most frequently takes place.
This question can be most conveniently answered by studying
the seasonal variation in the birthrate, calculating
back to the time of conception. Wargentin, in Sweden,
first called attention to the periodicity of the birthrate
in 1767.[151] The matter seems to have attracted little
further attention until Quetelet, who instinctively
scented unreclaimed fields of statistical investigation,
showed that in Belgium and Holland there is a maximum
of births in February, and, consequently, of conceptions
in May, and a minimum of births about July, with consequent
minimum of conceptions in October. Quetelet considered
that the spring maximum of conceptions corresponded
to an increase of vitality after the winter cold. He
pointed out that this sexual climax was better marked
in the country than in towns, and accounted for this
by the consideration that in the country the winter
cold is more keenly felt. Later, Wappaeus investigated
the matter in various parts of northern and southern
Europe as well as in Chile, and found that there was
a maximum of conceptions in May and June attributable
to season, and in Catholic countries strengthened by
customs connected with ecclesiastical seasons. This
maximum was, he found, followed by a minimum in September,
October, and November, due to gradually increasing exhaustion,
and the influence of epidemic diseases, as well as the
strain of harvest-work. The minimum is reached in the
south earlier than in the north. About November conceptions
again become more frequent, and reach the second maximum
at about Christmas and New Year. This second maximum
is very slightly marked in southern countries, but strongly
marked in northern countries (in Sweden the absolute
maximum of conceptions is reached in December), and
is due, in the opinion of Wappaeus, solely to social
causes. Villerme reached somewhat similar results. Founding
his study on 17,000,000 births, he showed that in France
it was in April, May, and June, or from the spring equinox
to the summer solstice, and nearer to the solstice than
the equinox, that the maximum of fecundations takes
place; while the minimum of births is normally in July,
but is retarded by a wet and cold summer in such a manner
that in August there are scarcely more births than in
July, and, on the other hand, a very hot summer, accelerating
the minimum of births, causes it to fall in June instead
of in July.[152] He also showed that in Buenos Ayres,
where the seasons are reversed, the conception-rate
follows the reversed seasons, and is also raised by
epochs of repose, of plentiful food, and of increased
social life. Sormani studied the periodicity of conception
in Italy, and found that the spring maximum in the southern
provinces occurs in May, and gradually falls later as
one proceeds northward, until, in the extreme north
of the peninsula, it occurs in July. In southern Italy
there is only one maximum and one minimum; in the north
there are two. The minimum which follows the spring
or summer maximum increases as we approach the south,
while the minimum associated with the winter cold increases
as we approach the north.[153] Beukemann, who studied
the matter in various parts of Germany, found that seasonal
influence was specially marked in the case of illegitimate
births. The maximum of conceptions of illegitimate children
takes place in the spring and summer of Europe generally;
in Russia it takes place in the autumn and winter, when
the harvest-working months for the population are over,
and the period of rest, and also of minimum deathrate
(September, October, and November), comes round. In
Russia the general conception-rate has been studied
by various investigators. Here the maximum number of
conceptions is in winter, the minimum varying among
different elements of the population. Looked at more
closely, there are maxima of conceptions in Russia in
January and in April. (In Russian towns, however, the
maximum number of conceptions occurs in the autumn.)
The special characteristics of the Russian conception-rate
are held to be due to the prevalence of marriages in
autumn and winter,[154] to the severely observed fasts
of spring, and to the exhausting harvest-work of summer.
It is instructive to compare the conception-rate of
Europe with that of a non-European country. Such a comparison
has been made by S.A. Hill for the Northwest Provinces
of India. Here the Holi and other erotic festivals take
place in spring; but spring is not the period when conceptions
chiefly take place; indeed, the prevalence of erotic
festivals in spring appears to Hill an argument in favor
of those festivals having originated in a colder climate.
The conceptions show a rise through October and November
to a maximum in December and January, followed by a
steady and prolonged fall to a minimum in September.
This curve can be accounted for by climatic and economic
conditions. September is near the end of the long and
depressing hot season, when malarial influences are
rapidly increasing to a maximum, the food-supply is
nearly exhausted, and there is the greatest tendency
to suicide. With October it forms the period of greatest
mortality. December, on the other hand, is the month
when food is most abundant, and it is also a very healthy
month.[155]
For a summary of the chief researches into this question,
see Ploss and Bartels, _Das Weib_; also, Rosenstadt,
"Zur Frage nach den Ursachen welche die Zahl der
Conceptionen, etc," _Mittheilungen aus den embryologischen
Institute Universitaet Wien_, second series, fasc. 4,
1890. Rosenstadt concludes that man has inherited from
animal ancestors a "physiological custom"
which has probably been further favored by climatic
and social conditions. "Primitive man," he
proceeds, "had inherited from his ancestors the
faculty of only reproducing himself at determined epochs.
On the arrival of this period of rut, fecundation took
place on a large scale, this being very easy, thanks
to the promiscuity in which primitive man lived. With
the development of civilization, men give themselves
up to sexual relations all the year around, but the
'physiological custom' of procreating at a certain epoch
has not completely disappeared; it remains as a survival
of the animal condition, and manifests itself in the
recrudescence of the number of conceptions during certain
months of the year." O. Rosenbach ("Bemerkungen
ueber das Problem einer Brunstzeit beim Menschen,"
_Archiv fuer Rassen und Gesellschafts-Biologie_, Bd.
III, Heft 5) has also argued in favor of a chief sexual
period in the year in man, with secondary and even tertiary
climaxes, in March, August, and December. He finds that
in some families, for several generations, birthdays
tend to fall in the same months, but his paper is, on
the whole, inconclusive.
Some years ago, Prof. J.B. Haycraft argued, on the basis
of data furnished by Scotland, that the conception-rate
corresponds to the temperature-curve (Haycraft, "Physiological
Results of Temperature Variation," _Transactions
of the Royal Society of Edinburgh_, vol. xxix, 1880).
"Temperature," he concluded, "is the
main factor regulating the variations in the number
of conceptions which occur during the year. It increases
their number with its elevation, and this on an average
of 0.5 per cent, for an elevation of 1 deg. F."
Whether or not this theory may fit the facts as regards
Scotland, it is certainly altogether untenable when
we take a broader view of the phenomena.
Recently Dr. Paul Gaedeken of Copenhagen has argued
in a detailed statistical study ("La Reaction de
l'Organisme sous l'Influence Physico-Chimiques des Agents
Meteorologiques," _Archives d'Anthropologie Criminelle_,
Feb., 1909) that the conception-rate, as well as the
periodicity of suicide and allied phenomena, is due
to the action of the chemical rays on the unpigmented
skin in early spring, this action being physiologically
similar to that of alcohol. He seeks thus to account
for the marked and early occurrence of such periodic
phenomena in Greenland and other northern countries
where there is much chemical action (owing to the clear
air) in early spring, but little heat. This explanation
would not cover an autumnal climax, the existence of
which Gaedeken denies.
In order to obtain a fairly typical conception-curve
for Europe, and to allow the variations of local habit
and custom to some extent to annihilate each other,
I have summated the figures given by Mayr for about
a quarter of a million births in Germany, France, and
Italy,[156] obtaining a curve (Chart 2) of the conception-rate
which may be said roughly to be that of Europe generally.
If we begin at September as the lowest point, we find
an autumn rise culminating in the lesser maximum of
Christmas, followed by a minor depression in January
and February. Then comes the great spring rise, culminating
in May, and followed after June by a rapid descent to
the minimum.
In Canada (see e.g., _Report of the Registrar General
of the Province of Ontario_ for 1904), the maximum and
minimum of conceptions alike fall later than in Europe;
the months of maximum conception are June, July, and
August; of minimum conception, January, February, and
March. June is the favorite month for marriage.
It would be of some interest to know the conception-curve
for the well-to-do classes, who are largely free from
the industrial and social influences which evidently,
to a great extent, control the conception-rate. It seems
probable that the seasonal influence would here be specially
well shown. The only attempt I have made in this direction
is to examine a well-filled birthday-book. The entries
show a very high and equally maintained maximum of conceptions
throughout April, May and June, followed by a marked
minimum during the next three months, and an autumn
rise very strongly marked, in November. There is no
December rise. As will be seen, there is here a fairly
exact resemblance to the yearly ecbolic curve of people
of the same class. The inquiry needs, however, to be
extended to a very much larger number of cases.
Mr. John Douglass Brown, of Philadelphia, has kindly
prepared and sent me, since the above was written, a
series of curves showing the, annual periodicity of
births among the educated classes in the State of Pennsylvania,
using the statistics as to 4,066 births contained in
the Biographical Catalogue of Matriculates of the College
of the University of Pennsylvania. Mr. Brown prepared
four curves: the first, covering the earliest period,
1757-1859; the second, the period 1860-1876; the third,
1877-1893; while the fourth presented the summated results
for the whole period. (The dates named are those of
the entry to classes, and not of actual occurrence of
birth.) A very definite and well-marked curve is shown,
and the average number of births (not conceptions) per
day, for the whole period, is as follows:--
Jan. Feb. Mar. Apr. May June July Aug. Sept. Oct. Nov.
Dec. 10.5 11.4 11 8.3 10.2 10.5 11.5 12.6 12.3 11.6
12 11.7
There is thus a well-marked minimum of conceptions (a
depression appearing here in each of the three periods,
separately) about the month of July. (In the second
period, however, which contains the smallest number
of births, the minimum occurs in September.) From that
low minimum there is steady and unbroken rise up to
the chief maximum in November. (In the first period,
however, the maximum is delayed till January, and in
the second period it is somewhat diffused.) There is
a tendency to a minor maximum in February, specially
well marked in the third and most important period,
and in the first period delayed until March.
A very curious and perhaps not accidental coincidence
might be briefly pointed out before we leave this part
of the subject. It is found[157] by taking 3000 cases
of children dying under one year that, among the general
population, children born in February and September
(and therefore conceived in May and December) appear
to possess the greatest vitality, and those born in
June, and, therefore, conceived in September, the least
vitality.[158] As we have seen, May and December are
precisely the periods when conceptions in Europe generally
are at a maximum, and September is precisely the period
when they are at a minimum, so that, if this coincidence
is not accidental, the strongest children are conceived
when there is the strongest tendency to procreate, and
the feeblest children when that tendency is feeblest.
Nelson, in his study of dreams and their relation to
seasonal ecbolic manifestations, does not present any
yearly ecbolic curve, as the two years and a half over
which his observations extend scarcely supply a sufficient
basis. On examining his figures, however, I find there
is a certain amount of evidence of a yearly rhythm.
There are spring and autumn climaxes throughout (in
February and in November); there is no December rise.
During one year there is a marked minimum from May to
September, though it is but slightly traceable in the
succeeding year. These figures are too uncertain to
prove anything, but, as far as they go, they are in
fair agreement with the much more extensive record,
that of W.K. (_ante_ p. 113), which I have already made
use of in discussing the question of a monthly rhythm.
This record, covering nearly twelve years, shows a general
tendency, when the year is divided into four periods
(November-January, February-April, May-July, August-October)
and the results summated, to rise steadily throughout,
from the minimum in the winter period to the maximum
in the autumn period. This steady upward progress is
not seen in each year taken separately. In three years
there is a fall in passing from the November-January
to the February-April quarter (always followed by a
rise in the subsequent quarter); in three cases there
is a fall in passing from the second to the third quarter
(again always followed by a rise in the following quarter),
and in two successive years there is a fall in passing
from the third to the fourth quarter. If, however, beginning
at the second year, we summate the results for each
year with those for all previous years, a steady rise
from season to season is seen throughout. If we analyze
the data according to the months of the year, still
more precise and interesting results (as shown in the
curve, Chart 3) are obtained; two maximum points are
seen, one in spring (March), one in autumn (October,
or, rather, August-October), and each of these maximum
points is followed by; a steep and sudden descent to
the minimum points in April and in December. If we compare
this result with Perry-Coste's also extending over a
long series of years, we find a marked similarity. In
both alike there are spring and autumn maxima, in both
the autumn maximum is the highest, and in both also
there is an intervening fall. In both cases, again,
the maxima are followed by steep descents, but while
in both the spring maximum occurs in March, in Perry-Coste's
case the second maximum, though of precisely similar
shape, occurs earlier, in June-September instead of
August-October. In Perry-Coste's case, also, there is
an apparently abnormal tendency, only shown in the more
recent years of the record, to an additional maximum
in January. The records certainly show far more points
of agreement than of discrepancy, and by their harmony,
as well with each other as with themselves, when the
years are taken separately, certainly go far to prove
that there is a very marked annual rhythm in the phenomena
of seminal emissions during sleep, or, as Nelson has
termed it, the ecbolic curve. We see, also, that the
great yearly organic climax of sexual effervescence
corresponds with the period following harvest, which,
throughout the primitive world, has been a season of
sexual erethism and orgy; though those customs have
died out of our waking lives, they are still imprinted
on our nervous texture, and become manifest during sleep.
The fresh records that have reached me since the first
edition of this book was published show well-marked
annual curves, though each curve always has some slight
personal peculiarities of its own. The most interesting
and significant is that of E.M. (see _ante_ p. 116),
covering four years. It is indicated by the following
monthly frequencies, summated for the four years:--
Jan. Feb. Mar. Apr. May June July Aug. Sept. Oct. Nov.
Dec. 16 13 14 22 19 19 12 12 14 14 12 24
E.M. lives in India. April, May, and June, are hot months,
but not unhealthy, and during this season, moreover,
he lives in the hills, under favorable conditions, getting
plenty of outdoor exercise. July, August, and September,
are nearly as hot, but much damper, and more trying;
during these months, E.M. is living in the city, and
his work is then, also, more exacting than at other
times, September is the worst month of all; he has a
short holiday at the end of it. During December, January,
and February, the climate is very fine, and E.M.'s work
is easier. It will be seen that his ecbolic curve corresponds
to his circumstances and environment, although until
he analyzed the record he had no idea that any such
relationship existed. Unfavorable climatic conditions
and hard work, favorable conditions and lighter work,
happen to coincide in his life, and the former depress
the frequency of seminal emissions; the latter increase
their frequency. At the same time, the curve is not
out of harmony with the northern curves. There is what
corresponds to a late spring (April) climax, and another
still higher, late autumn (December) climax. A very
interesting point is the general resemblance of the
ecbolic curves to the Indian conception-curves as set
forth by Hill (_ante_ p. 140). The conception-curve
is at its lowest point in September, and at its highest
point in December-January, and this ecbolic curve follows
it, except that both the minimum and the maximum are
reached a little earlier. When compared with the English
annual ecbolic curves (W.K. and Perry-Coste), both spring
and autumn maxima fall rather later, but all agree in
representing the autumn rise as the chief climax.
The annual curve of A.N. (_ante_ p. 117), who lives
in Indiana, U.S.A., also covers four years. It presents
the usual spring (May-June, in this case) and autumn
(September-October) climaxes. The exact monthly results,
summated for the four years, are given below; in order
to allow for the irregular lengths of the months, I
have reduced them to daily averages, for convenience
treating the four years as one year:--
Jan. Feb. Mar. Apr. May June July Aug. Sept. Oct. Nov.
Dec. 13 9 13 20 23 22 20 20 21 23 9 16 .42 .32 .42 .66
.74 .73 .64 .64 .70 .74 .30 .52
In his book on _Adolescence_, Stanley Hall refers to
three
ecbolic
records in his possession, all made by men who were
doctors of philosophy, and all considering themselves
normal. The best of these records made by "a virtuous,
active and able man," covered nearly eight years.
Stanley Hall thus summarizes the records, which are
not presented in detail: "The best of these records
averages about three and a half such experiences per
month, the most frequent being 5.14 for July, and the
least frequent 2.28, for September, for all the years
taken together. There appears also a slight rise in
April, and another in November, with a fall in December."
The frequency varies in the different individuals. There
was no tendency to a monthly cycle. In the best case,
the minimum number for the year was thirty-seven, and
the maximum, fifty. Fifty-nine per cent. of all were
at an interval of a week or less; forty per cent. at
an interval of from one to four days; thirty-four per
cent, at an interval of from eight to seventeen days,
the longest being forty-two days. Poor condition, overwork,
and undersleep, led to infrequency. Early morning was
the most common time. Normally there was a sense of
distinct relief, but in low conditions, or with over-frequency,
depression. (G.S. Hall, _Adolescence_, vol. i, p. 453.)
I may add that an anonymous article on "Nocturnal
Emissions" (_American Journal of Psychology_, Jan.,
1904) is evidently a fuller presentation of the first
of Stanley Hall's three cases. It is the history of
a healthy, unmarried, chaste man, who kept a record
of his nocturnal emissions (and their accompanying dreams)
from the age of thirty to thirty-eight. In what American
State he lived is not mentioned. He was ignorant of
the existence of any previous records. The yearly average
was 37 to 50, remaining fairly constant; the monthly
average was 3.43. I reproduce the total results summated
for the months, separately, and I have worked out the
daily average for each month, for convenience counting
the summated eight years as one year:--
Jan. Feb. Mar. Apr. May June July Aug. Sept. Oct. Nov.
Dec. 27 27 27 31 29 28 36 25 18 27 30 24 .87 .94 .87
1.03 .93 .93 1.16 .81 .60 .87 1.00 .77
Here, as in all the other curves we have been able to
consider, we may see the usual two points of climax
in spring and in autumn; the major climax covers April,
May, June, and July, the minor autumnal climax is confined
to November. In the light of the evidence which has
thus accumulated, we may conclude that the existence
of an annual ecbolic curve, with its spring and autumn
climaxes, as described in the first edition of this
book, is now definitely established.
If we are to believe, as these records tend to show,
that the nocturnal and involuntary voice of the sexual
impulse usually speaks at least as loudly in autumn
as in spring, we are confronted by a certain divergence
of the sleeping sexual impulse from the waking sexual
instinct, as witnessed by the conception-curve, and
also, it may be added, by the general voice of tradition,
and, indeed, of individual feeling, which concur, on
the whole, in placing the chief epoch of sexual activity
in spring and early summer, more especially as regards
women.[159] It is not impossible to reconcile the contradiction,
assuming it to be real, but I will refrain here from
suggesting the various explanations which arise. We
need a broader basis of facts.
There are many facts to show that early spring and,
to a certain extent, autumn are periods of visible excitement,
mainly sexual in character. We have already seen that
among the Eskimo menstruation and sexual desire occur
chiefly in spring, but cases are known of healthy women
in temperate climes who only menstruate twice a year,
and in such cases the menstrual epochs appear to be
usually in spring and autumn. Such, at all events, was
the case in a girl of 20, whose history has been recorded
by Dr. Mary Wenck, of Philadelphia.[160] She menstruated
first when 15 years old. Six months later the flow again
appeared for the second time, and lasted three weeks,
without cessation. Since then, for five years, she menstruated
during March and September only, each time for three
weeks, the flow being profuse, but not exhaustingly
so, without pain or systemic disturbance. Examination
revealed perfectly normal uterus and ovarian organs.
Treatment, accompanied by sitz-baths during the time
of month the flow should appear, accomplished nothing.
The semi-annual flow continued and the girl seemed in
excellent health.
It is a remarkable fact that, as noted by Dr. Hamilton
Wey at Elmira, sexual outbursts among prisoners appear
to occur at about March and October. "Beginning
with the middle of February," writes Dr. Wey in
a private letter, "and continuing for about two
months, is a season of ascending sexual wave; also the
latter half of September and the month of October. We
are now (March 30th) in the midst of a wave."
According to Chinese medicine, it is the spring which
awakens human passions. In early Greek tradition, spring
and summer were noted as the time of greatest wantonness.
"In the season of toilsome summer," says Hesiod
(_Works and Days_, xi, 569-90), "the goats are
fattest, wine is best, women most wanton, and men weakest."
It was so, also, in the experience of the Romans. Pliny
(_Natural History_, Bk. XII, Ch. XLIII) states that
when the asparagus blooms and the cicada sings loudest,
is the season when women are most amorous, but men least
inclined to pleasure. Paulus AEgineta said that hysteria
specially abounds during spring and autumn in lascivious
girls and sterile women, while more recent observers
have believed that hysteria is particularly difficult
to treat in autumn. Oribasius (_Synopsis_, lib. i, cap.
6) quotes from Rufus to the effect that sexual feeling
is most strong in spring, and least so in summer. Rabelais
said that it was in March that the sexual impulse is
strongest, referring this to the early warmth of spring,
and that August is the month least favorable to sexual
activity (_Pantagruel_, liv. v, Ch. XXIX). Nipho, in
his book on love dedicated to Joan of Aragon, discussed
the reasons why "women are more lustful and amorous
in summer, and men in winter." Venette, in his
_Generation de l'homme_, harmonized somewhat conflicting
statements with the observation that spring is the season
of love for both men and women; in summer, women are
more amorous than men; in autumn, men revive to some
extent, but are still oppressed by the heat, which,
sexually, has a less depressing effect on women. There
is probably a real element of truth in this view, and
both extremes of heat and cold may be regarded as unfavorable
to masculine virility. It is highly probable that the
well-recognized tendency of piles to become troublesome
in spring and in autumn, is due to increased sexual
activity. Piles are favored by congestion, and sexual
excitement is the most powerful cause of sudden congestion
in the genito-anal region. Erasmus Darwin called attention
to the tendency of piles to recur about the equinoxes
(_Zooenomia_, Section XXXVI), and since his days Gant,
Bonavia, and Cullimore have correlated this periodicity
with sexual activity.
Laycock, quoting the opinions of some earlier authorities
as to the prevalence of sexual feeling in spring, stated
that that popular opinion "appears to be founded
on fact" (_Nervous Diseases of Women_, p. 69).
I find that many people, and perhaps especially women,
confirm from their own experience, the statement that
sexual feeling is strongest in spring and summer. Wichmann
states that pollutions are most common in spring (being
perhaps the first to make that statement), and also
nymphomania. (In the eighteenth century, Schurig recorded
a case of extreme and life-long sexual desire in a woman
whose salacity was always at its height towards the
festival of St. John, _Gynaecologia_, p. 16.) A correspondent
in the Argentine Republic writes to me that "on
big estancias, where we have a good many shepherds,
nearly always married, or, rather, I should say, living
with some woman (for our standard of morality is not
very high in these parts), we always look out for trouble
in springtime, as it is a very common thing at this
season for wives to leave their husbands and go and
live with some other man." A corresponding tendency
has been noted even among children. Thus, Sanford Bell
("The Emotion of Love Between the Sexes,"
_American Journal Psychology_, July, 1902) remarks:
"The season of the year seems to have its effect
upon the intensity of the emotion of sex-love among
children. One teacher, from Texas, who furnished me
with seventy-six cases, said that he had noticed that
in the matter of love children seemed 'fairly to break
out in the springtime.' Many of the others who reported,
incidentally mentioned the love affairs as beginning
in the spring. This also agrees with my own observations."
Crichton-Browne remarks that children in springtime
exhibit restlessness, excitability, perversity, and
indisposition to exertion that are not displayed at
other times. This condition, sometimes known as "spring
fever," has been studied in over a hundred cases,
both children and adults, by Kline. The majority of
these report a feeling of tiredness, languor, lassitude,
sometimes restlessness, sometimes drowsiness. There
is often a feeling of suffocation, and a longing for
Nature and fresh air and day-dreams, while work seems
distasteful and unsatisfactory. Change is felt to be
necessary at all costs, and sometimes there is a desire
to begin some new plan of life.[161] In both sexes there
is frequently a wave of sexual emotion, a longing for
love. Kline also found by examination of a very large
number of cases that between the ages of four and seventeen
it is in spring that running away from home most often
occurs. He suggests that this whole group of phenomena
may be due to the shifting of the metabolic processes
from the ordinary grooves into reproductive channels,
and seeks to bring it into connection with the migrations
of animals for reproductive purposes.[162]
It has long been known that the occurrence of insanity
follows an annual curve,[163] and though our knowledge
of this curve, being founded on the date of admissions
to asylums, cannot be said to be quite precise, it fairly
corresponds to the outbreaks of acute insanity. The
curve presented in Chart 4 shows the admissions to the
London County Council Lunatic Asylums during the years
1893 to 1897 inclusive; I have arranged it in two-month
periods, to neutralize unimportant oscillations. In
order to show that this curve is not due to local or
accidental circumstances, we may turn to France and
take a special and chronic form of mental disease: Garnier,
in his _Folie a Paris_, presents an almost exactly similar
curve of the admissions of cases of general paralysis
to the Infirmerie Speciale at Paris during the years
1886-88 (Chart 5). Both curves alike show a major climax
in spring and a minor climax in autumn.
Crime in general in temperate climates tends to reach
its maximum at the beginning of the hot season, usually
in June. Thus, in Belgium, the minimum is in February;
the maximum in June, thence gradually diminishing (Lentz,
_Bulletin Societe Medecine Mentale Belgique_, March,
1901). In France, Lacassagne has summated the data extending
over more than 40 years, and finds that for all crimes
June is the maximum month, the minimum being reached
in November. He also gives the figures for each class
of crime separately, and every crime is found to have
its own yearly curve. Poisonings show a chief maximum
in May, with slow fall and a minor climax in December;
assassinations have a February and a November climax.
Parricides culminate in May-June, and in October (Lacassagne's
tables are given by Laurent, _Les Habitues des Prisons
de Paris_, Ch. 1).
Notwithstanding the general tendency for crime to reach
its maximum in the first hot month (a tendency not necessarily
due to the direct influence of heat), we also find,
when we consider the statistics of crime generally (including
sexual crime), that there is another tendency for minor
climaxes in spring and autumn. Thus, in Italy, Penta,
taking the statistics of nearly four thousand crimes
(murder, highway robbery, and sexual offences), found
the maximum in the first summer months, but there were
also minor climaxes in spring and in August and September
(Penta, _Rivista Mensile di Psichiatria_, 1899). In
nearly all Europe (as is shown by a diagram given by
Lombroso and Laschi, at the end of the first volume
of _Le Crime Politique_), while the chief climaxes occur
about July, there is, in most countries, a distinct
tendency to spring (usually about March) and autumn
(September and November) climaxes, though they rarely
rise as high as the July climax.
If we consider the separate periodicity of sexual offences,
we find that they follow the rule for crimes generally,
and usually show a chief maximum in early summer. Aschaffenburg
finds that the annual periodicity of the sexual impulse
appears more strongly marked the more abnormal its manifestations,
which he places in the following order of increasing
periodicity: conceptions in marriage, conceptions out
of marriage, offences against decency, rape, assaults
on children (_Centralblatt fuer Nervenheilkunde_, January,
1903). In France, rapes and offences against modesty
are most numerous in May, June, and July, as Villerme,
Lacassagne, and others have shown. Villerme, investigating
1,000 such cases, found a gradual ascent in frequency
(only slightly broken in March) to a maximum in June
(oscillating between May and July, when the years are
considered separately), and then a gradual descent to
a minimum in December. Legludic gives, for the 159 cases
he had investigated, a table showing a small February-March
climax, and a large June-August maximum, the minimum
being reached in November-January. (Legludic, _Attentats
aux Moeurs_, 1896, p. 16.) In Germany, Aschaffenburg
finds that sexual offences begin to increase in March
and April, reach a maximum in June or July, and fall
to a minimum in winter (_Monatsschrift fuer Psychiatrie_,
1903, Heft 2). In Italy, Penta shows that sexual offences
reach a minor climax in May (corresponding, in his experience,
with the maximum for crimes generally, as well as with
the maximum for conceptions), and a more marked climax
in August-September (Penta, _I Pervertimenti Sessuali_,
1893, p. 115; id. _Rivista Mensile di Psichiatria_,
1899).
Corre, in his _Crime en Pays Creole_, presents charts
of the seasonal distribution of crime in Guadeloupe,
with relation to temperature, which show that while,
in a mild temperature like that of France and England,
crime attains its maximum in the hot season, it is not
so in a more tropical climate; in July, when in Guadeloupe
the heat attains its maximum degree, crime of all kinds
falls suddenly to a very low minimum. Even in the United
States, where the summer heat is often excessive, it
tends to produce a diminution of crime.
Dexter, in an elaborate study of the relationship of
conduct to the weather, shows that in the United States
assaults present the maximum of frequency in April and
October, with a decrease during the summer and the winter.
"The unusual and interesting fact demonstrated
here with a certainty that cannot be doubted is,"
he concludes, "that the unseasonably hot days of
spring and autumn are the pugnacious ones, even though
the actual heat be much less than for summer. We might
infer from this that conditions of heat, up to a certain
extent, are vitalizing, while, at the same time, irritating,
but above that limit, heat is so devitalizing in its
effects as to leave hardly energy enough to carry on
a fight." (E.G. Dexter, _Conduct and the Weather_,
1899, pp. 63 _et seq._)
It is not impossible that the phenomena of seasonal
periodicity in crimes may possess a real significance
in relation to sexual periodicity. If, as is possible,
the occurrence of spring and autumn climaxes of criminal
activity is due less to any special exciting causes
at these seasons than to the depressing influences of
heat and cold in summer and winter, it may appear reasonable
to ask whether the spring and autumn climaxes of sexual
activity are not really also largely due to a like depressing
influence of extreme temperatures at the other two seasons.
Not only is there periodicity in criminal conduct, but
even within the normal range of good and bad conduct
seasonal periodicity may still be traced. In his _Physical
and Industrial Training of Criminals_, H.D. Wey gives
charts of the conduct of seven prisoners during several
years, as shown by the marks received. These charts
show that there is a very decided tendency to good behavior
during summer and winter, while in spring (February,
March, and April) and in autumn (August, September and
October) there are very marked falls to bad conduct,
each individual tending to adhere to a conduct-curve
of his own. Wey does not himself appear to have noticed
this seasonal periodicity. Marro, however, has investigated
this question in Turin on a large scale and reaches
results not very dissimilar from those shown by Wey's
figures in New York. He noted the months in which over
4,000 punishments were inflicted on prisoners for assaults,
insults, threatening language, etc., and shows the annual
curve in Tavola VI of his _Caratteri dei Delinquenti_.
There is a marked and isolated climax in May; a still
more sudden rise leads to the chief maximum of punishment
in August; and from the minimum in October there is
rapid ascent during the two following months to a climax
much inferior to that of May.
The seasonal periodicity of bad conduct in prisons is
of interest as showing that we cannot account for psychic
periodicity by invoking exclusively social causes. This
theory of psychic periodicity has been seriously put
forward, but has been investigated and dismissed, so
far as crime in Holland is concerned, by J.R.B. de Roos,
in the Transactions of the sixth Congress of Criminal
Anthropology, at Turin, in 1906 (_Archivio di Psichiatria_
fasc. 3, 1906).
The general statistics of suicides in Continental Europe
show a very regular and unbroken curve, attaining a
maximum in June and a minimum in December, the curve
rising steadily through the first six months, sinking
steadily through the last six months, but always reaching
a somewhat greater height in May than in July.[164]
Morselli shows that in various European countries there
is always a rise in spring and in autumn (October or
November).[165] Morselli attributes these spring and
autumn rises to the influence of the strain of the early
heat and the early cold.[166] In England, also, if we
take a very large number of statistics, for instance,
the figures for London during the twenty years between
1865 and 1884, as given by Ogle (in a paper read before
the Statistical Society in 1886), we find that, although
the general curve has the same maximum and minimum points,
it is interrupted by a break on each side of the maximum,
and these two breaks occur precisely at about March
and October.[167] This is shown in the curve in Chart
6, which presents the daily average for the different
months.
The growth of children follows an annual rhythm. Wahl,
the director of an educational establishment for homeless
girls in Denmark, who investigated this question, found
that the increase of weight for all the ages investigated
was constantly about 33 per cent. greater in the summer
half-year than in the winter half-year. It was noteworthy
that even the children who had not reached school-age,
and therefore could not be influenced by school-life,
showed a similar, though slighter, difference in the
same direction. It is, however, Malling-Hansen, the
director of an institution for deaf-mutes in Copenhagen,
who has most thoroughly investigated this matter over
a great many years. He finds that there are three periods
of growth throughout the year, marked off in a fairly
sharp manner, and that during each of these periods
the growth in weight and height shows constant characteristics.
From about the end of November up to about the end of
March is a period when growth, both in height and weight,
proceeds at a medium rate, reaching neither a maximum
nor a minimum; increase in weight is slight, the increase
in height, although trifling, preponderating. After
this follows a period during which the children show
a marked increase in height, while increase in weight
is reduced to a minimum. The children constantly lose
in weight during this period of growth in height almost
as much as they gain in the preceding period. This period
lasts from March and April to July and August. Then
follows the third period, which continues until November
and December. During this period increase in height
is very slight, being at its early minimum; increase
in weight, on the other hand, at the beginning of the
period (in September and October), is rapid and to the
middle of December very considerable, daily increase
in weight being three times as great as during the winter
months. Thus it may be said that the spring sexual climax
corresponds, roughly, with growth in height and arrest
of growth in weight, while the autumn climax corresponds
roughly with a period of growth in weight and arrest
of growth in height. Malling-Hansen found that slight
variations in the growth of the children were often
dependent on changes in temperature, in such a way that
a rise of temperature, even lasting for only a few days,
caused an increase of growth, and a fall of temperature
a decrease in growth. At Halle, Schmid-Monnard found
that nearly all growth in weight took place in the second
half of the year, and that the holidays made little
difference. In America, Peckham has shown that increase
of growth is chiefly from the 1st of May to the 1st
of September.[168] Among young girls in St. Petersburg,
Jenjko found that increase in weight takes place in
summer. Goepel found that increase in height takes place
mostly during the first eight months of the year, reaching
a maximum in August, declining during the autumn and
winter, in February being _nil_, while in March there
is sometimes loss in weight even in healthy children.
In the course of a study as to the consumption of bread
in Normal schools during each month of the year, as
illustrating the relationship between intellectual work
and nutrition, Binet presents a number of curves which
bring out results to which he makes no allusion, as
they are outside his own investigation. Almost without
exception, these curves show that there is an increase
in the consumption of bread in spring and in autumn,
the spring rise being in February, March, and April;
the autumn rise in October or November. There are, however,
certain fallacies in dealing with institutions like
Normal schools, where the conditions are not perfectly
regular throughout the year, owing to vacations, etc.
It is, therefore, instructive to find that under the
monotonous conditions of prison-life precisely the same
spring and autumn rises are found. Binet takes the consumption
of bread in the women's prison at Clermont, where some
four hundred prisoners, chiefly between the ages of
thirty and forty, are confined, and he presents two
curves for the years 1895 and 1896. The curves for these
two years show certain marked disagreements with each
other, but both unite in presenting a distinct rise
in April, preceded and followed by a fall, and both
present a still more marked autumn rise, in one case
in September and November, in the other case in October.[169]
Some years ago, Sir J. Crichton-Browne stated that a
manifestation of the sexual stimulus of spring is to
be found in the large number of novels read during the
month of March ("Address in Psychology" at
the annual meeting of the British Medical Association,
Leeds, 1889; _Lancet_, August 14, 1889). The statement
was supported by figures furnished by lending libraries,
and has since been widely copied. It would certainly
be interesting if we could so simply show the connection
between love and season, by proving that when the birds
began to sing their notes, the young person's fancy
naturally turns to brood over the pictures of mating
in novels. I accordingly applied to Mr. Capel Shaw,
Chief Librarian of the Birmingham Free Libraries (specially
referred to by Sir J. Crichton-Browne), who furnished
me with the Reports for 1896 and 1897-98 (this latter
report is carried on to the end of March, 1898).
The readers who use the Birmingham Free Lending Libraries
are about 30,000 in number; they consist very largely
of young people between the ages of 14 and 25; somewhat
less than half are women. Certainly we seem to have
here a good field for the determination of this question.
The monthly figures for each of the ten Birmingham libraries
are given separately, and it is clear at a glance that
without exception the maximum number of readers of prose-fiction
at all the libraries during 1897-98 is found in the
month of March. (I have chiefly taken into consideration
the figures for 1897-98; the figures for 1896 are somewhat
abnormal and irregular, probably owing to a decrease
in readers, attributed to increased activity in trade,
and partly to a disturbing influence caused by the opening
of a large new library in the course of the year, suddenly
increasing the number of readers, and drafting off borrowers
from some of the other libraries.) Not only so, but
there is a second, or autumnal climax, almost equaling
the spring climax, and occuring with equal certainty,
appearing during 1897-98 either in October or November,
and during 1896, constantly in October. Thus, the periodicity
of the rate of consumption of prose-fiction corresponds
with the periodicity which is found to occur in the
conception rate and in sexual ecbolic manifestations.
It is necessary, however, to examine somewhat more closely
the tables presented in these reports, and to compare
the rate of the consumption of novels with that of other
classes of literature. In the first place, if, instead
of merely considering the consumption of novels per
month, we make allowance for the varying length of the
months, and consider the average _daily_ consumption
per month, the supremacy of March at once vanishes.
February is really the month during which most novels
were read during the first quarter of 1898, except at
two libraries, where February and March are equal. The
result is similar if we ascertain the daily averages
for the first quarter in 1897, while, in 1896 (which,
however, as I have already remarked, is a rather abnormal
year), the daily average for March in many of the libraries
falls below that for January, as well as for February.
Again, when we turn to the other classes of books, we
find that this predominance which February possesses,
and to some extent shares with March and January, by
no means exclusively applies to novels. It is not only
shared by both music and poetry,--which would fit in
well with the assumption of a sexual _nisus_,--but the
department of "history, biography, voyages, and
travels" shares it also with considerable regularity;
so, also, does that of "arts, sciences, and natural
history," and it is quite well marked in "theology,
moral philosophy, etc.," and in "juvenile
literature." We even have to admit that the promptings
of the sexual instinct bring an increased body of visitors
to the reference library (where there are no novels),
for here, also, both the spring and autumnal climaxes
are quite distinct. Certainly this theory carries us
a little too far.
The main factor in producing this very marked annual
periodicity seems to me to be wholly unconnected with
the sexual impulse. The winter half of the year (from
the beginning of October to the end of March), when
outdoor life has lost its attractions, and much time
must be spent in the house, is naturally the season
for reading. But during the two central months of winter,
December and January, the attraction of reading meets
with a powerful counter-attraction in the excitement
produced by the approach of Christmas, and the increased
activity of social life which accompanies and for several
weeks follows Christmas. In this way the other four
winter months--October and November at the autumnal
end, and February and March at the spring end--must
inevitably present the two chief reading climaxes of
the year; and so the reports of lending libraries present
us with figures which show a striking, but fallacious,
resemblance to the curves which are probably produced
by more organic causes.
I am far from wishing to deny that the impulse which
draws young men and women to imaginative literature
is unconnected with the obscure promptings of the sexual
instinct. But, until the disturbing influence I have
just pointed out is eliminated, I see no evidence here
for any true seasonal periodicity. Possibly in prisons--the
value of which, as laboratories of experimental psychology
we have scarcely yet begun to realize--more reliable
evidence might be obtained; and those French and other
prisons where novels are freely allowed to the prisoners
might yield evidence as regards the consumption of fiction
as instructive as that yielded at Clermont concerning
the consumption of bread.
Certain diseases show a very regular annual curve. This
is notably the case with scarlet fever. Caiger found
in a London fever hospital a marked seasonal prevalence:
there was a minor climax in May (repeated in July),
and a great autumnal climax in October, falling to a
minimum in December and January. This curve corresponds
closely to that usually observed in London.[170] It
is not peculiar to London, or to urban districts, for
in rural districts we find nearly the same spring minor
maximum and major autumnal maximum. In Russia it is
precisely the same. Many other epidemic diseases show
very similar curves.
An annual curve may be found in the expulsive force
of the bladder as measured by the distance to which
the urinary stream can be projected. This curve, as
ascertained for one case, is interesting on account
of the close relationship between sexual and vesical
activity. After a minimum point in autumn there is a
rise through the early part of the year to a height
maintained through spring and summer, and reaching its
maximum in August.[171] This may be said to correspond
with the general tendency found in some cases of nocturnal
seminal emissions from a winter minimum to an autumn
maximum.
There is an annual curve in voluntary muscle strength.
Thus in Antwerp, where the scientific study of children
is systematically carried out by a Pedological Bureau,
Schuyten found that, measured by the dynamometer, both
at the ages of 8 and 9, both boys and girls showed a
gradual increase of strength from October to January,
a fall from January to March and a rise to June or July.
March was the weakest month, June and July the strongest.[172]
Schuyten also found an annual curve for mental ability,
as tested by power of attention, which for much of the
year corresponded to the curve of muscular strength,
being high during the cold winter months. Lobsien, at
Kiel, seeking to test Schuyten's results and adopting
a different method so as to gauge memory as well as
attention, came to conclusions which confirmed those
of Schuyten. He found a very marked increase of ability
in December and January, with a fall in April; April
and May were the minimum months, while July and October
also stood low.[173] The inquiries of Schuyten and Lobsien
thus seem to indicate that the voluntary aptitudes of
muscular and mental force in children reach their maximum
at a time of the year when most of the more or less
involuntary activities we have been considering show
a minimum of energy. If this conclusion should be confirmed
by more extended investigations, it would scarcely be
matter for surprise and would involve no true contradiction.
It would, indeed, be natural to suppose that the voluntary
and regulated activities of the nervous system should
work most efficiently at those periods when they are
least exposed to organic and emotional disturbance.
So persistent a disturbing element in spring and autumn
suggests that some physiological conditions underlie
it, and that there is a real metabolic disturbance at
these times of the year. So few continuous observations
have yet been made on the metabolic processes of the
body that it is not easy to verify such a surmise with
absolute precision. Edward Smith's investigations, so
far as they go, support it, and Perry-Coste's long-continued
observations of pulse-frequency seem to show with fair
regularity a maximum in early spring and another maximum
in late autumn.[174] I may also note that Haig, who
has devoted many years of observations to the phenomena
of uric-acid excretion, finds that uric acid tends to
be highest in the spring months, (March, April, May)
and lowest at the first onset of cold in October.[175]
Thus, while the sexual climaxes of spring and autumn
are rooted in animal procreative cycles which in man
have found expression in primitive festivals--these,
again, perhaps, strengthening and developing the sexual
rhythm--they yet have a wider significance. They constitute
one among many manifestations of spring and autumn physiological
disturbance corresponding with fair precision to the
vernal and autumnal equinoxes. They resemble those periods
of atmospheric tension, of storm and wind, which accompany
the spring and autumn phases in the earth's rhythm,
and they may fairly be regarded as ultimately a physiological
reaction to those cosmic influences.
FOOTNOTES:
[128] F. Smith, _Veterinary Physiology_; Dalziel, _The
Collie_.
[129] Mondiere, Art "Cambodgiens," _Dictionnaire
des Sciences Anthropologiques_.
[130] This primitive aspect of the festival is well
shown by the human sacrifices which the ancient Mexicans
offered at this time, in order to enable the sun to
recuperate his strength. The custom survives in a symbolical
form among the Mokis, who observe the festivals of the
winter solstice and the vernal equinox. ("Aspects
of Sun-worship among the Moki Indians," _Nature_,
July 28, 1898.) The Walpi, a Tusayan people, hold a
similar great sun-festival at the winter solstice, and
December is with them a sacred month, in which there
is no work and little play. This festival, in which
there is a dance dramatizing the fructification of the
earth and the imparting of virility to the seeds of
corn, is fully described by J. Walter Fewkes (_American
Anthropologist_, March, 1898). That these solemn annual
dances and festivals of North America frequently merge
into "a lecherous _saturnalia_" when "all
is joy and happiness," is stated by H.H. Bancroft
(_Native Races of Pacific States_, vol. i, p. 352).
[131] As regards the northern tribes of Central Australia,
Spencer and Gillen state that, during the performance
of certain ceremonies which bring together a large number
of natives from different parts, the ordinary marital
rules are more or less set aside (_Northern Tribes of
Central Australia_, p. 136). Just in the same way, among
the Siberian Yakuts, according to Sieroshevski, during
weddings and at the great festivals of the year, the
usual oversight of maidens is largely removed. (_Journal
of the Anthropological Institute_, Jan.-June, 1901,
p. 96.)
[132] R.E. Guise, _Journal of the Anthropological Institute_,
1899, pp. 214-216.
[133] Dalton, _Ethnology of Bengal_, pp. 196 et seq.
W. Crooke (_Journal of the Anthropological Institute_,
p. 243, 1899) also refers to the annual harvest-tree
dance and _saturnalia_, and its association with the
seasonal period for marriage. We find a similar phenomenon
in the Malay Peninsula: "In former days, at harvest-time,
the Jakuns kept an annual festival, at which, the entire
settlement having been called together, fermented liquor,
brewed from jungle fruits, was drunk; and to the accompaniments
of strains of their rude and incondite music, both sexes,
crowning themselves with fragrant leaves and flowers,
indulged in bouts of singing and dancing, which grew
gradually wilder throughout the night, and terminated
in a strange kind of sexual orgie." (W.W. Skeat,
"The Wild Tribes of the Malay Peninsula,"
_Journal of the Anthropological Institute_, 1902, p.
133.)
[134] Fielding Hall, _The Soul of a People_, 1898, Chapter
XIII.
[135] See e.g., L. Dyer, _Studies of the Gods in Greece_,
1891, pp. 86-89, 375, etc.
[136] For a popular account of the Feast of Fools, see
Loliee, "La Fete des Fous," _Revue des Revues_,
May 15, 1898; also, J.G. Bourke, _Scatologic Rites of
all Nations_, pp. 11-23.
[137] J. Grimm (_Teutonic Mythology_, p. 615) points
out that the observance of the spring or Easter bonfires
marks off the Saxon from the Franconian peoples. The
Easter bonfires are held in Lower Saxony, Westphalia,
Lower Hesse, Geldern, Holland, Friesland, Jutland, and
Zealand. The Midsummer bonfires are held on the Rhine,
in Franconia, Thuringia, Swabia, Bavaria, Austria, and
Silesia. Schwartz (_Zeitschrift fuer Ethnologie_, 1896,
p. 151) shows that at Lauterberg, in the Harz Mountains,
the line of demarcation between these two primitive
districts may still be clearly traced.
[138] _Wald und Feldkulte_, 1875, vol. i, pp. 422 et
seq. He also mentions (p. 458) that St. Valentine's
Day (14th of February),--or Ember Day, or the last day
of February,--when the pairing of birds was supposed
to take place, was associated, especially in England,
with love-making and the choice of a mate. In Lorraine,
it may be added, on the 1st of May, the young girls
chose young men as their valentines, a custom known
by this name to Rabelais.
[139] Rochholz, _Drei gaugoettinnen_, p, 37.
[140] Mannhardt, ibid., pp. 466 et seq. Also J.G. Frazer,
_Golden Bough_, vol ii, Chapter IV. For further facts
and references, see K. Pearson (_The Chances of Death_,
1897, vol, ii, "Woman as Witch," "Kindred
Group-marriage," and Appendix on "The '_Mailehn_'
and '_Kiltgang_,'") who incidentally brings together
some of the evidence concerning primitive sex-festivals
in Europe. Also, E. Hahn, _Demeter und Baubo_, 1896,
pp. 38-40; and for some modern survivals, see Deniker,
_Races of Man_, 1900, Chapter III. On a lofty tumulus
near the megalithic remains at Carnac, in Brittany,
the custom still prevails of lighting a large bonfire
at the time of the summer solstice; it is called Tan
Heol, or Tan St. Jean. In Ireland, the bonfires also
take place on St. John's Eve, and a correspondent, who
has often witnessed them in County Waterford, writes
that "women, with garments raised, jump through
these fires, and conduct which, on ordinary occasions
would be reprobated, is regarded as excusable and harmless."
Outside Europe, the Berbers of Morocco still maintain
this midsummer festival, and in the Rif they light bonfires;
here the fires seem to be now regarded as mainly purificatory,
but they are associated with eating ceremonies which
are still regarded as multiplicative. (Westermarck,
"Midsummer Customs in Morocco," _Folk-Lore_,
March, 1905.)
[141] Mannhardt (op. cit., p. 469) quotes a description
of an Ehstonian festival in the Island of Moon, when
the girls dance in a circle round the fire, and one
of them,--to the envy of the rest, and the pride of
her own family,--is chosen by the young men, borne away
so violently that her clothes are often torn, and thrown
down by a youth, who places one leg over her body in
a kind of symbolical coitus, and lies quietly by her
side till morning. The spring festivals of the young
people of Ukrainia, in which, also, there is singing,
dancing, and sleeping together, are described in "Folk-Lore
de l'Ukrainie." Kryptadia, vol. v, pp. 2-6, and
vol. viii, pp. 303 et seq.
[142] M. Kowalewsky, "Marriage Among the Early
Slavs," _Folk-Lore_, December, 1890.
[143] A. Tille, however (_Yule and Christmas_, 1899),
while admitting that the general Aryan division of the
year was dual, follows Tacitus in asserting that the
Germanic division of the year (like the Egyptian) was
tripartite: winter, spring, and summer.
[144] Grimm, _Teutonic Mythology_ (English translation
by Stallybrass), pp. 612-630, 779, 788.
[145] Wellhausen, _Reste Arabischen Heidentums_, 1897,
p. 98.
[146] See, e.g., the chapter on ritual in Gerard-Varet's
interesting book, _L'Ignorance et l'Irreflexion_, 1899,
for a popular account of this and allied primitive conceptions.
[147] Jastrow, _Religion of Babylonia_, especially pp.
485, 571; regarding the priestesses, Jastrow remarks:
"Among many nations, the mysterious aspects of
woman's fertility lead to rites that, by a perversion
of their original import, appear to be obscene. The
prostitutes were priestesses attached to the Ishtar
cult, and who took part in ceremonies intended to symbolize
fertility." Whether there is any significance in
the fact that the first two months of the Babylonian
year (roughly corresponding to our March and April),
when we should expect births to be at a maximum, were
dedicated to Ea and Bel, who, according to varying legends,
were the creators of man, and that New Year's Day was
the festival of Bau, regarded as the mother of mankind,
I cannot say, but the suggestion may be put forward.
[148] _Celtic Heathendom_, p. 421.
[149] Grimm, _Teutonic Mythology_, p. 1465. In England,
the November, bonfires have become merged into the Guy
Fawkes celebrations. In the East, the great primitive
autumn festivals seem to have fallen somewhat earlier.
In Babylonia, the seventh month (roughly corresponding
to September) was specially sacred, though nothing is
known of its festivals, and this also was the sacred
festival month of the Hebrews, and originally of the
Arabs. In Europe, among the southern Slavs, the Reigen,
or Kolo--wild dances by girls, adorned with flowers,
and with skirts girt high, followed by sexual intercourse--take
place in autumn, during the nights following harvest
time.
[150] A. Tille, _Yule and Christmas_, p. 21, etc.
[151] Long before Wargentin, however, Rabelais had shown
some interest in this question, and had found that there
were most christenings in October and November, this
showing, he pointed out, that the early warmth of spring
influenced the number of conceptions (_Pantagruel_,
liv. v, Ch. XXIX). The spring maximum of conceptions
is not now so early in France.
[152] Villerme, "De la Distribution par mois des
conceptions," _Annales d'Hygiene Publique_, tome
v, 1831, pp. 55-155.
[153] Sormani, _Giornale di Medicina Militare_, 1870.
[154] Throughout Europe, it may be said, marriages tend
to take place either in spring or autumn (Oettinger
_Moralstatistik_, p. 181, gives details). That is to
say, that there is a tendency for marriages to take
place at the season of the great public festivals, during
which sexual intercourse was prevalent in more primitive
times.
[155] Hill, _Nature_, July 12, 1888.
[156] G. Mayr, _Die Gesetzmaessigkeit im Gesellschaftsleben_,
1877, p. 240.
[157] Edward Smith (_Health and Disease_), who attributes
this to the lessened vitality of offspring at that season.
Beukemann also states that children born in September
have most vitality.
[158] Westermarck has even suggested that the December
maximum of conceptions may be due to better chance of
survival for September offspring (_Human Marriage_,
Chapter II). It may be noted that though the maximum
of conceptions is in May, relatively the smallest proportion
of boys is conceived at that time. (Rauber, _Der Ueberschuss
an Knabengeburten_, p. 39.)
[159] Krieger found that the great majority of German
women investigated by him menstruated for the first
time in September, October, or November. In America,
Bowditch states that the first menstruation of country
girls more often occurs in spring than at any other
season.
[160] _Women's Medical Journal_, 1894.
[161] It is, perhaps, worth while noting that the wisdom
of the mediaeval Church found an outlet for this "spring
fever" in pilgrimages to remote shrines. As Chaucer
wrote, in the _Canterbury Tales_:--
"Whane that Aprille with his showers sote The droughts
of March hath pierced to the root, Thaen longen folk
to gon on pilgrimages, And palmers for to seeken strange
stronds."
[162] L.W. Kline, "The Migratory Impulse,"
_American Journal of Psychology_, 1898, vol. x, especially
pp. 21-24.
[163] Mania comes to a crisis in spring, said the old
physician, Aretaeus (Bk. 1, Ch. V).
[164] This is, at all events, the case in France, Prussia,
and Italy. See, for instance, Durkheim's discussion
of the cosmic factors of suicide, _Le Suicide_, 1897,
Chapter III. In Spain, as Bernaldo de Quiros shows (_Criminologia_,
p. 69), there is a slight irregular rise in December,
but otherwise the curve is perfectly regular, with maximum
in June, and minimum in January.
[165] This holds good of a south European country, taken
separately. A chart of the annual incidence of suicide
by hanging, in Roumania, presented by Minovici (_Archives
d'Anthropologie Criminelle_, 1905, p. 587), shows climaxes
of equal height in May and September.
[166] Morselli, _Suicide_, pp. 55-72.
[167] Ogle himself was inclined to think that these
breaks were accidental, being unaware of the allied
phenomena with which they may be brought into line.
It is true that (as Gaedeken objects to me) the autumnal
break is very slight, but it is probably real when we
are dealing with so large a mass of data.
[168] _Pedagogical Seminary_, June, 1891, p. 298. For
a very full summary and bibliography of investigations
regarding growth, see F. Burk, "Growth of Children
in Height and Weight," _American Journal of Psychology_,
April, 1898.
[169] _L'Annee Psychologique_, 1898.
[170] _Lancet_, June 6, 1891. Edward Smith had pointed
out many years earlier that scarlet fever is most fatal
in periods of increasing vitality.
[171] Havelock Ellis, "The Bladder as a Dynamometer,"
_American Journal of Dermatology_, May, 1902.
[172] See, e.g., summary in _Internationales Centrablatt
fuer Anthropologie_, 1902, Heft 4, p. 207.
[173] Summarized in _Zeitschrift fuer Psychologie der
Sinnesorgane_, 1903, p. 135.
[174] Camerer found that from September to November
is the period of greatest metabolic activity.
[175] Haig, _Uric Acid_, 6th edition, 1903, p. 33.
|