II.
The Question of a Monthly Sexual Cycle in Men--The Earliest
Suggestions of a General Physiological Cycle in Men--Periodicity
in Disease--Insanity, Heart Disease, etc.--The Alleged
Twenty-three Days' Cycle--The Physiological Periodicity
of Seminal Emissions during Sleep--Original Observations--Fortnightly
and Weekly Rhythms.
For some centuries, at least, inquisitive observers here
and there have thought they found reason to believe that
men, as well as women, present various signs of a menstrual
physiological cycle. It would be possible to collect a
number of opinions in favor of such a monthly physiological
periodicity in men. Precise evidence, however, is, for
the most part, lacking. Men have expended infinite ingenuity
in establishing the remote rhythms of the solar system
and the periodicity of comets. They have disdained to
trouble about the simpler task of proving or disproving
the cycles of their own organisms.[117] It is over half
a century since Laycock wrote that "the _scientific_
observation and treatment of disease are impossible without
a knowledge of the mysterious revolutions continually
taking place in the system"; yet the task of summarizing
the whole of our knowledge regarding these "mysterious
revolutions" is even to-day no heavy one. As to the
existence of a monthly cycle in the sexual instincts of
men, with a single exception, I am not aware that any
attempt has been made to bring forward definite evidence.[118]
A certain interest and novelty attaches, therefore, to
the evidence I am able to produce, although that evidence
will not suffice to settle the question finally.
The great Italian physician, Sanctorius, who was in so
many ways the precursor of our modern methods of physiological
research by the means of instruments of precision, was
the first, so far as I am aware, to suggest a monthly
cycle of the organism in men. He had carefully studied
the weight of the body with reference to the amount of
excretions, and believed that a monthly increase in weight
to the amount of one or two pounds occurred in men, followed
by a critical discharge of urine, this crisis being preceded
by feelings of heaviness and lassitude.[119] Gall, another
great initiator of modern views, likewise asserted a monthly
cycle in men. He insisted that there is a monthly critical
period, more marked in nervous people than in others,
and that at this time the complexion becomes dull, the
breath stronger, digestion more laborious, while there
is sometimes disturbance of the urine, together with general
_malaise_, in which the temper takes part; ideas are formed
with more difficulty, and there is a tendency to melancholy,
with unusual irascibility and mental inertia, lasting
a few days. More recently Stephenson, who established
the cyclical wave-theory of menstruation, argued that
it exists in men also, and is really "a general law
of vital energy."[120]
Sanctorius does not appear to have published the data
on which his belief was founded. Keill, an English, follower
of Sanctorius, in his _Medicina Statica Britannica_ (1718),
published a series of daily (morning and evening) body-weights
for the year, without referring to the question of a monthly
cycle. A period of maximum weight is shown usually, by
Keill's figures, to occur about once a month, but it is
generally irregular, and cannot usually be shown to occur
at definite intervals. Monthly discharges of blood from
the sexual organs and other parts of the body in men have
been recorded in ancient and modern times, and were treated
of by the older medical writers as an affliction peculiar
to men with a feminine system. (Laycock, _Nervous Diseases
of Women_, p. 79.) A summary of such cases will be found
in Gould and Pyle (_Anomalies and Curiosities of Medicine_,
1897, pp. 27-28). Laycock (_Lancet_, 1842-43, vols. i
and ii) brought forward cases of monthly and fortnightly
cycles in disease, and asserted "the general principle
that there are greater and less cycles of movements going
on in the system, involving each other, and closely connected
with the organization of the individual." He was
inclined to accept lunar influence, and believed that
the physiological cycle is made up of definite fractions
and multiples of a period of seven days, especially a
unit of three and a half days. Albrecht, a somewhat erratic
zooelogist, put forth the view a few years ago that there
are menstrual periods in men, giving the following reasons:
(1) males are rudimentary females, (2) in all males of
mammals, a rudimentary masculine uterus (Mueller's ducts)
still persists, (3) totally hypospadic male individuals
menstruate; and believed that he had shown that in man
there is a rudimentary menstruation consisting in an almost
monthly periodic appearance, lasting for three or four
days, of white corpuscles in the urine (_Anomalo_, February,
1890). Dr. Campbell Clark, some years since, made observations
on asylum attendants in regard to the temperature, during
five weeks, which tended to show that the normal male
temperature varies considerably within certain limits,
and that "so far as I have been able to observe,
there is one marked and prolonged rise every month or
five weeks, averaging three days, occasional lesser rises
appearing irregularly and of shorter duration. These observations
are only made in three cases, and I have no proof that
they refer to the sexual appetite" (Campbell Clark,
"The Sexual Reproductive Functions," Psychological
Section, British Medical Association, Glasgow, 1888; also,
private letters). Hammond (_Treatise on Insanity_, p.
114) says: "I have certainly noted in some of my
friends, the tendency to some monthly periodic abnormal
manifestations. This may be in the form of a headache,
or a nasal haemorrhage, or diarrhoea, or abundant discharge
of uric acid, or some other unusual occurrence. I think,"
he adds, "this is much more common than is ordinarily
supposed, and a careful examination or inquiry will generally,
if not invariably, establish the existence of a periodicity
of the character referred to."
Dr. Harry Campbell, in his book on _Differences in the
Nervous Organization of Men and Women_, deals fully with
the monthly rhythm (pp. 270 et seq.), and devotes a short
chapter to the question, "Is the Menstrual Rhythm
peculiar to the Female Sex?" He brings forward a
few pathological cases indicating such a rhythm, but although
he had written a letter to the _Lancet_, asking medical
men to supply him with evidence bearing on this question,
it can scarcely be said that he has brought forward much
evidence of a convincing kind, and such as he has brought
forward is purely pathological. He believes, however,
that we may accept a monthly cycle in men. "We may,"
he concludes, "regard the human being--both male
and female--as the subject of a monthly pulsation which
begins with the beginning of life, and continues till
death," menstruation being regarded as a function
accidentally ingrafted upon this primordial rhythm.
It is not unreasonable to argue that the possibility of
such a menstrual cycle is increased, if we can believe
that in women, also, the menstrual cycle persists even
when its outward manifestations no longer occur. Aetius
said that menstrual changes take place during gestation;
in more modern times, Buffon was of the same opinion.
Laycock also maintained that menstrual changes take place
during pregnancy (_Nervous Diseases of Women_, p. 47).
Fliess considers that it is certainly incorrect to assert
that the menstrual process is arrested during pregnancy,
and he refers to the frequency of monthly epistaxis and
other nasal symptoms throughout this period (W. Fliess,
_Beziehungen zwischen Nase und Geschlechts-Organen_, pp.
44 et seq.). Beard, who attaches importance to the persistence
of a cyclical period in gestation, calls it the muffled
striking of the clock. Harry Campbell (_Causation of Disease_,
p. 54) has found post-climacteric menstrual rhythm in
a fair sprinkling of cases up to the age of sixty.
It is somewhat remarkable that, so far as I have observed,
none of these authors refer to the possibility of any
heightening of the sexual appetite at the monthly crisis
which they believe to exist in men. This omission indicates
that, as is suggested by the absence of definite statements
on the matter of increase of sexual desire at menstruation,
it was an ignored or unknown fact. Of recent years, however,
many writers, especially alienists, have stated their
conviction that sexual desire in men tends to be heightened
at approximately monthly intervals, though they have not
always been able to give definite evidence in support
of their statements.
Clouston, for instance, has frequently asserted this monthly
periodic sexual heightening in men. In the article, "Developmental
Insanity," in Tuke's _Psychological Dictionary_,
he refers to the periodic physiological heightening of
the reproductive _nisus_; and, again, in an article on
"Alternation, Periodicity, and Relapse in Mental
Diseases" (_Edinburgh Medical Journal_, July, 1882),
he records the case of "an insane gentleman, aged
49, who, for the past twenty-six years, has been subject
to the most regularly occurring brain-exaltation every
four weeks, almost to a day. It sometimes passes off without
becoming acutely maniacal, or even showing itself in outward
acts; at other times it becomes so, and lasts for periods
of from one to four weeks. It is always preceded by an
uncomfortable feeling in the head, and pain in the back,
mental hebetude, and slight depression. The _nisus generativus_
is greatly increased, and he says that, if in that condition,
he has full and free seminal emissions during sleep, the
excitement passes off; if not, it goes on. A full dose
of bromide or iodide of potassium often, but not always,
has the effect of stopping the excitement, and a very
long walk sometimes does the same. When the excitement
gets to a height, it is always followed by about a week
of stupid depression." In the same article Clouston
remarks: "I have for a long time been impressed with
the relationship of the mental and bodily alternations
and periodicities in insanity to the great physiological
alternations and periodicities, and I have generally been
led to the conclusion that they are the same in all essential
respects, and only differ in degree of intensity or duration.
By far the majority of the cases in women follow the law
of the menstrual and sexual periodicity; the majority
of the cases in men follow the law of the more irregular
periodicities of the _nisus generativus_ in that sex.
Many of the cases in both sexes follow the seasonal periodicity
which perhaps in man is merely a reversion to the seasonal
generative activities of the majority of the lower animals."
He found that among 338 cases of insanity, chiefly mania
and melancholia, 46 per cent, of females and 40 per cent,
of males showed periodicity,--diurnal, monthly, seasonal,
or annual, and more marked in women than in men, and in
mania than in melancholia,--and adds: "I found that
the younger the patient, the greater is the tendency to
periodic remission and relapse. The phenomenon finds its
acme in the cases of pubescent and adolescent insanity."
Conolly Norman, in the article "Mania, Hysterical"
(Tuke's _Psychological Dictionary_), states that "the
activity of the sexual organs is probably in both sexes
fundamentally periodic."
Krafft-Ebing records the case of a neurasthenic Russian,
aged 24, who experienced sexual desires of urologinic
character, with fair regularity, every four weeks (_Psychopathia
Sexualis_), and Naecke mentions the case of a man who
had nocturnal emissions at intervals of four weeks (_Archiv
fuer Kriminal-Anthropologie_, 1908, p. 363), while Moll
(_Libido Sexualis_, Bd. I, pp. 621-623) recorded the case
of a man, otherwise normal, who had attacks of homosexual
feeling every four weeks, and Rohleder (_Zeitschrift fuer
Sexualwissenschaft_, Nov., 1908) gives the case of an
unmarried slightly neuropathic physician who for several
days every three to five weeks has attacks of almost satyriacal
sexual excitement.
Fere, whose attention was called to this point, from time
to time noted the existence of sexual periodicity. Thus,
in a case of general paralysis, attacks of continuous
sexual excitement, with sleeplessness, occurred every
twenty-eight days; at other times, the patient, a man
of 42, in the stage of dementia, slept well, and showed
no signs of sexual excitation (_Societe de Biologie_,
October 6, 1900). In another case, of a man of sound heredity
and good health till middle life, periodic sexual manifestations
began from puberty, with localized genital congestion,
erotic ideas, and copious urination, lasting for two or
three days. These manifestations became menstrual, with
a period of intermenstrual excitement appearing regularly,
but never became intense. Between the age of 36 and 42,
the intermenstrual crises gradually ceased; at about 45,
the menstrual crises ceased; the periodic crises continued,
however, with the sole manifestation of increased frequency
of urination (_Societe de Biologie_, July 23, 1904). In
a third case, of sexual neurasthenia, Fere found that
from puberty, onwards to middle life, there appeared,
every twenty-five to twenty-eight days, tenderness and
swelling below the nipple, accompanied by slight sexual
excitation and erotic dreams, lasting for one or two days
(_Revue de Medecine_, March, 1905).
It is in the domain of disease that the most strenuous
and, on the whole, the most successful efforts have been
made to discover a menstrual cycle in men. Such a field
seems promising at the outset, for many morbid exaggerations
or defects of the nervous system might be expected to
emphasize, or to free from inhibition, fundamental rhythmical
processes of the organism which in health, and under the
varying conditions of social existence, are overlaid by
the higher mental activities and the pressure of external
stimuli. In the eighteenth century Erasmus Darwin wrote
a remarkable and interesting chapter on "The Periods
of Disease," dealing with solar and lunar influence
on biological processes.[121] Since then, many writers
have brought forward evidence, especially in the domain
of nervous and mental disease, which seems to justify
a belief that, under pathological conditions, a tendency
to a male menstrual rhythm may be clearly laid bare.
We should expect an organ so primitive in character as
the heart, and with so powerful a rhythm already stamped
upon its nervous organization, to be peculiarly apt to
display a menstrual rhythm under the stress of abnormal
conditions. This expectation might be strengthened by
the menstrual rhythm which Mr. Perry-Coste has found reason
to suspect in pulse-frequency during health. I am able
to present a case in which such a periodicity seems to
be indicated. It is that of a gentleman who suffered severely
for some years before his death from valvular disease
of the heart, with a tendency to pulmonary congestion,
and attacks of "cardiac asthma." His wife, a
lady of great intelligence, kept notes of her husband's
condition,[122] and at last observed that there was a
certain periodicity in the occurrence of the exacerbations.
The periods were not quite regular, but show a curious
tendency to recur at about thirty days' interval, a few
days before the end of every month; it was during one
of these attacks that he finally died. There was also
a tendency to minor attacks about ten days after the major
attacks. It is noteworthy that the subject showed a tendency
to periodicity when in health, and once remarked laughingly
before his illness: "I am just like a woman, always
most excitable at a particular time of the month."
Periodicity has been noted in various disorders of nervous
character. Periodic insanity has long been known and studied
(see, e.g., Pilcz, _Die periodischen Geistesstoerungen_,
1901); it is much commoner in women than in men. Periodicity
has been observed in stammering (a six-weekly period in
one case), and notably in hemicrania or migraine, by Harry
Campbell, Osler, etc. (The periodicity of a case of hemicrania
has been studied in detail by D. Fraser Harris, _Edinburgh
Medical Journal_, July, 1902.) But the cycle in these
cases is not always, or even usually, of a menstrual type.
It is now possible to turn to an investigation which,
although of very limited extent, serves to place the question
of a male menstrual cycle for the first time on a sound
basis. If there is such a cycle analogous to menstruation
in women, it must be a recurring period of nervous erethism,
and it must be demonstrably accompanied by greater sexual
activity. In the _American Journal of Psychology_ for
1888, Mr. Julius Nelson, afterward Professor of Biology
at the Rutgers College of Agriculture, New Brunswick,
published a study of dreams in which he recorded the results
of detailed observations of his dreams, and also of seminal
emissions during sleep (by him termed "gonekbole"
or "ecbole"), during a period of something over
two years. Mr. Nelson found that both dreams and ecboles
fell into a physiological cycle of 28 days. The climax
of maximum dreaming (as determined by the number of words
in the dream record) and the climax of maximum ecbole
fell at the same point of the cycle, the ecbolic climax
being more distinctly marked than the dream climax.
The question of cyclic physiological changes is considerably
complicated by our uncertainty regarding the precise length
of the cycle we may expect to find. Nelson finds a 28-day
cycle satisfactory. Perry-Coste, as we shall see, accepts
a strictly lunar cycle of 291/2 days. Fliess has argued
that in both women and men, many physiological facts fall
into a cycle of 23 days, which he calls male, the 28-day
cycle being female. (W. Fliess, _Die Beziehungen zwischen
Nase und weiblichen Geschlechts-Organen_, 1897, pp. 113
et seq.) Although Fliess brings forward a number of minutely-observed
cases, I cannot say that I am yet convinced of the reality
of this 23-day cycle. It is somewhat curious, however,
that at the same time as Fliess, though in apparent independence,
and from a different point of view, another worker also
suggested that there is a 23-day physiological cycle (John
Beard, _The Span of Gestation and the Cause of Birth_,
Jena, 1897). Beard approaches the question from the embryological
standpoint, and argues that there is what he terms an
"ovulation unit" of about 231/2 days, in the
interval from the end of one menstruation to the beginning
of the next. Two "ovulation units" make up one
"critical unit," and the length of pregnancy,
according to Beard, is always a multiple of the "critical
unit;" in man, the gestation period amounts to six
critical units. These attempts to prove a new physiological
cycle deserve careful study and further investigation.
The possibility of such a cycle should be borne in mind,
but at present we are scarcely entitled to accept it.
So far as I am aware, Professor Nelson's very interesting
series of observations, which, for the first time, placed
the question of a menstrual rhythm in men on a sound and
workable basis, have not directly led to any further observations.
I am, however, in possession of a much more extended series
of ecbolic observations completed before Nelson's paper
was published, although the results have only been calculated
at a comparatively-recent date. I now propose to present
a summary of these observations, and consider how far
they confirm Nelson's conclusions. These observations
cover no less a period than twelve years, between the
ages of 17 and 29, the subject, W.K., being a student,
and afterward schoolmaster, leading, on the whole, a chaste
life. The records were faithfully made throughout the
whole of this long period. Here, if anywhere, should be
material for the construction of a menstrual rhythm on
an ecbolic basis. While the results are in many respects
instructive, it can scarcely, perhaps, be said that they
absolutely demonstrate a monthly cycle. When summated
in a somewhat similar manner to that adopted by Nelson
in his ecbolic observations, it is not difficult to regard
the maximum, which is reached on the 19th to 21st days
of the summated physiological month, as a real menstrual
ecbolic climax, for no other three consecutive days at
all approach these in number of ecboles, while there is
a marked depression occurring four days earlier, on the
16th day of the month. If, however, we split up the curve
by dividing the period of twelve years into two nearly
equal periods, the earlier of about seven years and the
latter of about four years, and summate these separately,
the two curves do not present any parallel as regards
the menstrual cycle. It scarcely seems to me, therefore,
that these curves present any convincing evidence in this
case of a monthly ecbolic cycle (and, therefore, I refrain
from reproducing them), although they seem to suggest
such a cycle. Nor is there any reason to suppose that
by adopting a different cycle of thirty days, or of twenty-three
days, any more conclusive results would be obtained.
It seems, however, when we look at these curves more closely,
that they are not wholly without significance. If I am
justified in concluding that they scarcely demonstrate
a monthly cycle, it may certainly be added that they show
a rudimentary tendency for the ecboles to fall into a
fortnightly rhythm, and a very marked and unmistakable
tendency to a weekly rhythm. The fortnightly rhythm is
shown in the curve for the earlier period, but is somewhat
disguised in the curve for the total period, because the
first climax is spread over two days, the 7th and 8th
of the month. If we readjust the curve for the total period
by presenting the days in pairs, the fortnightly tendency
is more clearly brought out (Chart I).
A more pronounced tendency still is traceable to a weekly
rhythm. This is, indeed, the most unquestionable fact
brought out by these curves. All the maxima occur on Saturday
or Sunday, with the minima on Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday,
or Friday. This very pronounced weekly rhythm will serve
to swamp more or less completely any monthly rhythm on
a 28-day basis. Although here probably seen in an exaggerated
form, it is almost certainly a characteristic of the ecbolic
curve generally.[123] I have been told by several young
men and women, especially those who work hard during the
week, that Saturday, and especially Sunday afternoon,
are periods when the thoughts spontaneously go in an erotic
direction, and at this time there is a special tendency
to masturbation or to spontaneous sexual excitement. It
is on Friday, Saturday, Sunday, and Monday, according
to Guerry's tables,[124] that the fewest suicides are
committed, Tuesday, Wednesday, and Thursday, with, however,
a partial fall on Wednesday, those on which most suicides
are committed, so that there would appear to be an antagonism
between sexual activity and the desire to throw off life.
It also appears (in the reports of the Bavarian factory
inspectors) that accidents in factories have a tendency
to occur chiefly at the beginning of the week, and toward
the end rather than in the middle.[125] Even growth, as
Fleischmann has shown in the case of children, tends to
fall into weekly cycles. It is evident that the nervous
system is profoundly affected by the social influences
resulting from the weekly cycle.
The analysis of this series of ecbolic curves may thus
be said to recall the suggestion of Laycock, that the
menstrual cycle is really made up of four weekly cycles,
the periodic unit, according to Laycock, being three and
one-half days. I think it would, however, be more correct
to say that the menstrual cycle, perhaps originally formed
with reference to the influence of the moon on the sexual
and social habits of men and other animals, tends to break
up by a process of segmentation into fortnightly and weekly
cycles. If we are justified in assuming that there is
a male menstrual cycle, we must conclude that in such
a case as that just analyzed, the weekly rhythm has become
so marked as almost entirely to obliterate the larger
monthly rhythm.
However constituted, there seems little doubt that a physiological
weekly cycle really exists. This was, indeed, very clearly
indicated many years ago by the observations of Edward
Smith, who showed that there are weekly rhythms in pulse,
respiration, temperature, carbonic acid evolution, urea,
and body-weight, Sunday being the great day of repair
and increase of weight.[126]
In an appendix to this volume I am able to present the
results of another long series of observations of nocturnal
ecbolic manifestations carried out by Mr. Perry-Coste,
who has elaborately calculated the results, and has convinced
himself that on the basis of a strictly lunar month, thus
abolishing the disturbing influence of the weekly rhythm,
which in his case also appears, a real menstrual rhythm
may be traced.[127]
It does not appear to me, however, even yet, that a final
answer to the question whether a menstrual sexual rhythm
occurs in men can be decisively given in the affirmative.
That such a cycle will be proved in many cases seems to
me highly probable, but before this can be decisively
affirmed it is necessary that a much larger number of
persons should be induced to carry out on themselves the
simple, but protracted, series of observations that are
required.
Since the first edition of this volume appeared, numerous
series of ecbolic records have reached me from different
parts of the world. The most notable of these series comes
from a professional man, of scientific training, who has
for the past six years lived in different parts of India,
where the record was kept. Though the record extends over
nearly six years, there are two breaks in it, due to a
visit to England, and to loss of interest. Both involuntary
and voluntary discharges are included in the record. The
involuntary discharges occurred during sleep, usually
with an erotic dream, in which the subject invariably
awaked and frequently made an effort to check the emission.
The voluntary discharges in most cases commenced during
sleep, or in the half-waking state; deliberate masturbation,
when fully awake, was comparatively rare. The proportion
of involuntary to more or less voluntary ecboles was about
3 to 1. A third kind of sexual manifestation (of frequency
intermediate between the other two forms) is also included,
in which a high degree of erethism is induced during the
half waking state, culminating in an orgasm in which the
power of preventing discharge has been artificially acquired.
The subject, E.M., was 32 years of age when the record
began. He belongs to a healthy family, and is himself
physically sound, 5 feet 6 inches in height, but weight
low, due to rickets in infancy. In early life he stammered
badly; his temperament is emotional and self-conscious,
while his work is unusually exacting, and he lives for
most of the year in a very trying climate. As a boy he
was very religious, and has always felt obliged to resist
sexual vice to the utmost, though there have been occasional
lapses.
As regards lunar periodicity, E.M., has summated his results
in a curve, after the same manner as Mr. Perry-Coste,
beginning with the new moon. The periods covered include
54 lunar months, and the total number of discharges is
176; the average frequency is about 3 per month of twenty-eight
days. The curve, for the most part, zigzags between a
frequency of 4 and 9, but on the twenty-fourth day it
falls to 1, and then rises uninterruptedly to a height
of 11 on the twenty-seventh day, falling to 2 on the next
day. Whether a really menstrual rhythm is thus indicated
I do not undertake to decide, but I am inclined to agree
with E.M. himself that there is no definite evidence of
it. "It looks to me," he writes, "as if
the only real rhythm (putting aside the annual cycle)
will be found to be the average period between the ecboles,
varying in different persons, but in my case, about nine
and one-eighth days. May not the ecbolic period in men
be compared to the menstrual period in women, and be an
example of the greater katabolic activity of men? There
is the period of tumescence, and the ecbole constituting
the detumescence. The week-end holiday would hasten the
detumescence, but about every third week-end there would
tend to be delay to enable the system to get back into
its regulation nine or ten days' stride. This might possibly
be the explanation of the curves. The recent emissions
were nearly all involuntary during sleep. Age may have
something to do with the change in character."
E.M.'s curves frequently show the influence of weekly
periodicity, in the tendency to ecbole on Sunday, or sometimes
on Saturday or Monday. In recent years there has been
some tendency for this climax to be thrown towards the
middle of the week, but, on the whole, Wednesday is the
point of lowest frequency.
In another case, the subject, A.N., who has spent nearly
all his life in the State of Indiana, has kept a record
of sexual manifestations between the ages of 30 and 34.
The data, which cover four years, have not been sent to
me in a form which enables the possibility of a monthly
curve to be estimated, but A.N., who has himself arranged
the data on a lunar monthly basis, considers that a monthly
curve is thus revealed. "My memoranda," he writes,
"show that discharges occur most frequently on the
first, second, and third days after new moon. There is
also another period on the fourteenth and fifteenth, which
might indicate a semi-lunar rhythm. The days of minimum
discharge are the seventh, eighth, twenty-second, and
twenty-third." It may be added that the yearly average
of ecbolic manifestations, varying between 50 and 55,
comes out as 52, or exactly one per week.
A weekly periodicity is very definitely shown by A.N.'s
data. Sunday once more stands at the head of the week
as regards frequency, in this case very decisively. The
figures are as follows:--
Sun. Mon. Tues. Wed. Thurs. Fri. Sat. 48 21 24 35 28 26
27
In another case which has reached me from the United States,
the data are slighter, but deserve note, as the subject
is a trained psychologist, and I quote the case in his
own words. Here, it will be seen, there appears to be
a tendency for the ecbolic cycle to cover a period of
about six weeks. In this case, also, there is a tendency
for the climax to occur about Saturday or Sunday. "X.
is 38 years old, unmarried, fair health, pretty good heredity;
university trained, and engaged in academic pursuits.
He thinks he may have completed puberty at about 13, though
he has no proof that he was in the full possession of
his sex-powers until he was 15 years 3 months old (when
he had his first emission). His sex life has been normal.
He masturbated somewhat when he slept with other boys
(or men) during early manhood, but not to excess.
"During the autumn of 1889 (when 28 years of age)
he observed that at certain times he had an itching feeling
about the testicles; that he felt slightly irritable;
that the penis erected with the slightest provocation,
and that this peculiar feeling usually passed away with
a nightly emission. Indeed, so regular was the matter
that he usually wore a loin garment at these times, to
prevent the semen getting on the bedding. This peculiar
feeling ordinarily continued for two or three days. He
recalls at these times that he felt that he would like
to wrestle with some one, for there seemed to be a muscular
tension. These states returned with apparent regularity,
and the intervals seemed to be about six weeks, though
no effort was made to measure the periods until 1893.
The following notes are taken from the diaries of X.:--
"Thursday, December 29, 1892. The peculiar feeling.
(This is the only entry.)
"Thursday, February 9, 1893. The peculiar feeling.
(The diary notes that X. awoke nights to find erections,
and that the feeling continued until Sunday night following,
when there was an emission.)
"Friday, March 27, 1893. The peculiar feeling. (The
diary notes that there was an emission the next night,
and that the feeling disappeared.)
"Wednesday, May 3, 1893. The peculiar feeling. (The
diary notes that it continued until Saturday night, when
X. had sexual relations, and that it then disappeared.)
"Wednesday, June 14, 1893. The peculiar feeling.
(The diary states that the next night X. had an emission,
and the disappearance of the feeling.)
"Thursday, July 27, 1893. The peculiar feeling. (The
diary notes that it was apparent at about 3 o'clock that
afternoon. That night at 10 o'clock, X. had sexual intercourse,
and the feeling was not noted the next day.)
"Friday, September 8, 1893. The peculiar feeling.
(Continued until Tuesday, the 11th, and then disappeared.
No sexual intercourse, and no nightly emission.)
"Wednesday, October 25, 1893. The peculiar feeling.
(Continued until Saturday night, when there was a nightly
emission.)
"Saturday, December 9, 1893. The peculiar feeling.
(Continued until Monday night, when there was sexual relations.)
"It will be noted that the intervals observed were
of about six weeks' duration, excepting one, that from
September to October, when it was nearly seven weeks.
"These observations were not recorded after 1893.
X. thinks that in 1894 the intervals were longer, an opinion
which is based on the fact that for a period of six months
he had no sexual intercourse and no nightly emissions.
The times during this six months when he had the 'peculiar
feeling,' the sensation was so slight as to be scarcely
noted. In 1895, the feeling seemed more pronounced than
ever before, and X. thinks that it may have recurred as
often as once a month. In 1896, 1897, and 1898, the intervals,
he thinks, lengthened--at times, he thought, wholly disappeared.
During 1899, while they did not recur often, when they
did come the sensation was pronounced, although the emission
was less common. There was a peculiar 'heavy' feeling
about the testicles, and a marked tendency towards erection
of the penis, especially at night-time (while sleeping).
X. often awoke to find a tense erection. Moreover, these
feelings usually continued a week.
"1. In general, X. is of the opinion that as he grows
older these intervals lengthen, though this inference
is not based on _recorded_ data.
"2. He notes that a discharge (through sexual intercourse
or in sleep) invariably brings the peculiar feeling to
a close for the time being.
"3. He notes that sexual intercourse _at the time_
stops it; but, when there has been sexual intercourse
within a week or ten days of the time (based upon the
observations of 1893), that it had no tendency to check
the feeling."
In another case, that of F.C., an Irish farmer, born in
Waterford, the data are still more meagre, though the
periodicity is stated to be very pronounced. He is chaste,
steady, with occasional lapses from strict sobriety, healthy
and mentally normal, living a regular open-air life, far
from the artificial stimuli of towns. The observations
refer to a period when he was from 20 to 27 years of age.
During this period, nocturnal emissions occurred at regular
intervals of exactly a month. They were ushered in by
fits of irritability and depression, and usually occurred
in dreamless sleep. The discharges were abundant and physically
weakening, but they relieved the psychic symptoms, though
they occasioned mental distress, since F.C. is scrupulous
in a religious sense, and also apprehensive of bad constitutional
effects, the result of reading alarmist quack pamphlets.
In another case known to me, a young man leading a chaste
life, experienced crises of sexual excitement every ten
to fourteen days, the crisis lasting for several days.
Finally, an interesting contribution to this subject,
suggested by this _Study_, has been made and published
(in the proceedings of the Amsterdam International Congress
of Psychology, in 1907) by the well-known Amsterdam neurologist
and psychologist, Dr. L.S.A.M. Von Roemer under the title,
"Ueber das Verhaeltniss zwischen Mondalter und Sexualitaet."
Von Roemer's data are made up not of nocturnal involuntary
emissions, but of the voluntary acts of sexual intercourse
of an unmarried man, during a period of four years. Von
Roemer believes that these, to a much greater extent than
those of a married man, would be liable to periodic influence,
if such exist. On making a curve of exact lunar length
(similarly to Perry-Coste), he finds that there are, every
month, two maxima and two minima, in a way that approximately
resemble Perry-Coste's curve. The main point in Von Roemer's
results is, however, the correspondence that he finds
with the actual lunar phases; the chief maximum occurs
at the time of the full moon, and the secondary maximum
at the time of the new moon, the minima being at the first
and fourth quarters. He hazards no theory in explanation
of this coincidence, but insists on the need for further
observations. It will be seen that A.N.'s results (_ante_
p. 117) seem in the main to correspond to Von Roemer's.
FOOTNOTES:
[117] Even counting the pulse is a comparatively recent
method of physiological examination. It was not until
1450 that Nicolas of Cusa advocated counting the pulse-beats.
(Binz, _Deutsche medizinische Wochenschrift_, October
6, 1898.)
[118] I leave this statement as it stands, though since
the first publication of this book it has ceased to be
strictly accurate.
[119] Sanctorius, _Medicina Statica_, Sect. I, aph. lxv.
[120] _American Journal of Obstetrics_, xiv, 1882.
[121] _Zooenomia_, Section XXXVI.
[122] I reproduced these notes in full in earlier editions
of this volume.
[123] Moll refers to the case of a man whose erotic dreams
occurred every fortnight, and always on Friday night (_Libido
Sexualis_, Band I, p. 136). One is inclined to suspect
an element of autosuggestion in such a case; still, the
coincidence is noteworthy.
[124] See Durkheim, _Le Suicide_, p. 101.
[125] We must, of course, see here the results of the
disorganization produced by holidays, and the exhaustion
produced by the week's labor; but such influences are
still the social effects of the cosmic week.
[126] E. Smith, _Health and Disease_, Chapter III. I may
remark that, according to Kemsoes (_Deutsche medizinische
Wochenschrift_, January 20, 1908, and _British Medical
Journal_, January 29, 1898), school-children work best
on Monday and Tuesday.
[127] See Appendix B. |
|